用户名: 密码: 验证码:
Efficiency of Lexical Access in Children with Autism Spectrum Disorders: Does Modality Matter?
详细信息    查看全文
  • 作者:Keely Harper-Hill (1) (2)
    David Copland (1) (2)
    Wendy Arnott (2)
  • 关键词:Children ; Language ; Semantic priming ; Retrospective semantic matching ; Attention ; Visual support ; Autism spectrum disorders
  • 刊名:Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders
  • 出版年:2014
  • 出版时间:August 2014
  • 年:2014
  • 卷:44
  • 期:8
  • 页码:1819-1832
  • 全文大小:416 KB
  • 参考文献:1. Australian Bureau of Statistics. (2006). 2033.0.55.001 census of population and housing: Socio-economic indexes for area (SEIFA), Australia—Data only 2006. www.abs.gov.au/ausstats/abs@nsf/mf/2033.0.55.001/. Accessed July 10, 2012.
    2. Balota, D. A., Yap, M. J., Cortese, M. J., Hutchison, K. A., Kessler, B., Loftis, B., et al. (2007). The English lexicon project. / Behavior Research Methods, / 39(3), 445-59. CrossRef
    3. Behrmann, M., Thomas, C., & Humphreys, K. (2006). Seeing it differently: Visual processing in autism. / Trends in Cognitive Sciences, / 10(6), 258-64. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2006.05.001 . CrossRef
    4. Bonnotte, I., & Casalis, S. (2010). Semantic priming in French children with varying comprehension skills. / European Journal of Developmental Psychology, / 7(3), 309-28. doi:10.1080/17405620802114546 . CrossRef
    5. Brand-D’Abrescia, M., & Lavie, N. (2008). Task coordination between and within sensory modalities: Effects on distraction. / Perception and Psychophysics, / 70(3), 508-15. doi:10.1038/42947 . CrossRef
    6. Brock, J., Norbury, C., Einav, S., & Nation, K. (2008). Do individuals with autism process words in context? Evidence from language-mediated eye-movements. / Cognition, / 108(3), 896-04. doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2008.06.007 . CrossRef
    7. Bruneau, N., Bonnet-Brilhault, F., Gomot, M., Adrien, J. L., & Barthelemy, C. (2003). Cortical auditory processing and communication in children with autism: electrophysiological/behavioral relations. / International Journal of Psychophysiology, / 51(1), 17-5. doi:10.1016/s0167-8760(03)00149-1 . CrossRef
    8. Bruneau, N., Roux, S., Adrien, J. L., & Barthelemy, C. (1999). Auditory associative cortex dysfunction in children with autism: evidence from late auditory evoked potentials (N1 wave-T complex). / Clinical Neurophysiology, / 110(11), 1927-934. CrossRef
    9. Brysbaert, M. (2007). / The language-as-fixed-effect-fallacy: Some simple SPSS solutions to a complex problem. London: Royal Holloway, University of London.
    10. Carlesimo, G. A., Turriziani, P., Paulesu, E., Gorini, A., Caltagirone, C., Fazio, F., et al. (2004). Brain activity during intra- and cross-modal priming: New empirical data and review of the literature. / Neuropsychologia, / 42(1), 14-4. doi:10.1016/s0028-3932(03)00148-9 . CrossRef
    11. Carlesimo, G. A., Vicari, S., Albertoni, A., Turriziani, P., & Caltagirone, C. (2000). Developmental dissociation between visual and auditory repetition priming: The role of input lexicons. / Cortex, / 36(2), 181-93. doi:10.1016/s0010-9452(08)70523-9 . CrossRef
    12. Castles, A., Holmes, V. M., Neath, J., & Kinoshita, S. (2003). How does orthographic knowledge influence performance on phonological awareness tasks? / The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology Section A, Human Experimental Psychology, / 56, 445-67. CrossRef
    13. Ceponiene, R., Lepisto, T., Shestakova, A., Vanhala, R., Alku, P., Naatanen, R., et al. (2003). Speech-sound-selective auditory impairment in children with autism: They can perceive but do not attend. / Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, / 100(9), 5567-572. CrossRef
    14. Cree, G. S., McRae, K., & McNorgan, C. (1999). An attractor model of lexical conceptual processing: Simulating semantic priming. / Cognitive Science, / 23(3), 371-14. CrossRef
    15. de Groot, A. M. B., Thomassen, A. J. W. M., & Hudson, P. T. W. (1986). Primed-lexical decision: The effect of varying the stimulus-onset asynchrony of prime and target. / Acta Psychologica, / 61(1), 17-6. doi:10.1016/0001-6918(86)90019-3 . CrossRef
    16. DeLemos, M. M. (1994). / Standard progressive matrices; Australian manual. Melbourne: Australian Council for Educational Research.
    17. Dunn, L. M., & Dunn, L. (1997). / The peabody picture vocabulary test—Revised (3rd ed.). Circle Pines, MI: American Guidance Service.
    18. Dunn, M., Vaughan, H., Kreuzer, J., & Kurtzberg, D. (1999). Electrophysiologic correlates of semantic classification in autistic and normal children. / Developmental Neuropsychology, / 16(1), 79-9. CrossRef
    19. Eskes, G. A., Bryson, S. E., & McCormick, T. A. (1990). Comprehension of concrete and abstract words in autistic children. / Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, / 20(1), 61-3. CrossRef
    20. Forsey, J., Bird, E. K.-R., & Bedrosian, J. (1996). Brief report: The effects of typed and spoken modality combinations on the language performance of adults with autism. / Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, / 26(6), 643-49. doi:10.1007/BF02172353 . CrossRef
    21. Fritz, J. B., Elhilali, M., David, S. V., & Shamma, S. A. (2007). Auditory attention—Focusing the searchlight on sound. / Current Opinion in Neurobiology, / 17(4), 437-55. doi:10.1016/j.conb.2007.07.011 . CrossRef
    22. Gardiner, J. M., Bowler, D. M., & Grice, S. J. (2003). Further evidence of preserved priming and impaired recall in adults with Asperger’s syndrome. / Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, / 33(3), 259-69. CrossRef
    23. Girbau, D., & Schwartz, R. G. (2011). Implicit semantic priming in Spanish-speaking children and adults: An auditory lexical decision task. / Spanish Journal of Psychology, / 14(1), 4-9. doi:10.5209/rev_SJOP.2011.v14.n1.1 . CrossRef
    24. Hala, S., Pexman, P. M., & Glenwright, M. (2007). Priming the meaning of homographs in typically developing children and children with autism. / Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, / 37(2), 329-40. doi:10.1007/s10803-006-0162-6 . CrossRef
    25. Harris, M., & Coltheart, M. (1986). / Language processing in children and adults: An introduction. London: Routledge & Kegan Paul.
    26. Hashimoto, N., McGregor, K. K., & Graham, A. (2007). Conceptual organization at 6 and 8?years of age: Evidence from the semantic priming of object decisions. / Journal of Speech Language and Hearing Research, / 50(1), 161-76. doi:10.1044/1092-4388(2007/014 . CrossRef
    27. Henderson, L. M., Clarke, P. J., & Snowling, M. J. (2011). Accessing and selecting word meaning in autism spectrum disorder. / Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, / 52(9), 964-73. doi:10.1111/j.1469-7610.2011.02393.x . CrossRef
    28. Hodgdon, L. (1999). / Visual strategies for improving communication. Troy MI: QuirkRoberts Publishing.
    29. Howlin, P. (1997). Prognosis in autism: Do specialist treatments affect long-term outcome? / European Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, / 6(2), 55-2. doi:10.1007/BF00566668 . CrossRef
    30. Joseph, R. M., Ehrman, K., McNally, R., & Keehn, B. (2008). Affective response to eye contact and face recognition ability in children with ASD. / Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, / 14(6), 947-55. doi:10.1017/s1355617708081344 . CrossRef
    31. Kail, R. (1992). Processing speed, speech rate, and memory. / Developmental Psychology, / 28(5), 899-04. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.28.5.899 . CrossRef
    32. Kamio, Y., Robins, D., Kelley, E., Swainson, B., & Fein, D. (2007). Atypical lexical/semantic processing in high-functioning autism spectrum disorders without early language delay. / Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, / 37(6), 1116-122. doi:10.1007/s10803-006-0254-3 . CrossRef
    33. Kamio, Y., & Toichi, M. (2000). Dual access to semantics in autism: Is pictorial access superior to verbal access. / Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, / 41(7), 859-67. CrossRef
    34. Klin, A. (1991). Young autistic children’s listening preferences in regard to speech: A possible characterisation of the symptom of social withdrawal. / Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 21(1), 29-2. CrossRef
    35. Lord, C., Rutter, M., DiLavore, P., & Risi, S. (1998). / Autism diagnostic observation schedule manual. Los Angeles: Western Psychological Service.
    36. Lukas, S., Philipp, A., & Koch, I. (2010a). Switching attention between modalities: Further evidence for visual dominance. / Psychological Research PRPF, / 74(3), 255-67. doi:10.1007/s00426-009-0246-y . CrossRef
    37. Lukas, S., Philipp, A. M., & Koch, I. (2010b). The role of preparation and cue-modality in cross modal task switching. / Acta Psychologica, / 134(3), 318-22. doi:10.1016/j.actpsy.2010.03.004 . CrossRef
    38. Manis, F. R., Keating, D. P., & Morrison, F. J. (1980). Developmental differences in the allocation of processing capacity. / Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, / 29(1), 156-69. doi:10.1016/0022-0965(80)90098-3 . CrossRef
    39. Manly, T., Robertson, I. H., Anderson, V., & Nimmo-Smith, I. (1999). / The test of everyday attention in children. Bury St. Edmunds: Thames Valley Test Company.
    40. Marcus, L. F., Garfinkle, A., & Wolery, M. (2001). Issues in early diagnosis and intervention with young children. In E. Schopler, N. Yirmiya, C. Shulman, & L. M. Marcus (Eds.), / The research basis for autism intervention (pp. 171-85). New York: Plenum Publishers.
    41. Marinelli, C. V., Angelelli, P., Di Filippo, G., & Zoccolotti, P. (2011). Is developmental dyslexia modality specific? A visual-auditory comparison of Italian dyslexics. / Neuropsychologia, / 49(7), 1718-729. doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2011.02.050 . CrossRef
    42. Martens, V. E. G., & de Jong, P. F. (2006). The effect of word length on lexical decision in dyslexic and normal reading children. / Brain and Language, / 98(2), 140-49. doi:10.1016/j.bandl.2006.04.003 . CrossRef
    43. McCleery, J. P., Ceponiene, R., Burner, K. M., Townsend, J., Kinnear, M., & Schreibman, L. (2010). Neural correlates of verbal and nonverbal semantic integration in children with autism spectrum disorders. / Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, / 51(3), 277-86. doi:10.1111/j.1469-7610.2009.02157.x . CrossRef
    44. McRae, K., & Boisvert, S. (1998). Automatic semantic similarity priming. / Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, / 24(3), 558-72. CrossRef
    45. McRae, K., Cree, G., Seidenberg, M., & McNorgan, C. (2005). Semantic feature production norms for a large set of living and nonliving things. / Behavior Research Methods, / 37(4), 547-59. doi:10.3758/bf03192726 . CrossRef
    46. Mirenda, P. (2001). Autism, augmentative communication, and assistive technology: What do we really know? / Focus on Autism and Other Developmental Disabilities, / 16(3), 141-51. CrossRef
    47. Morrison, C. M., Chappell, T. D., & Ellis, A. W. (1997). Age of acquisition norms for a large set of object names and their relation to adult estimates and other variables. / Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology Section A—Human Experimental Psychology, / 50(3), 528-59. CrossRef
    48. Nation, K., & Snowling, M. J. (1998). Semantic processing and the development of word-recognition skills: Evidence from children with reading comprehension difficulties. / Journal of Memory and Language, / 39(1), 85-01. doi:10.1006/jmla . 1998.2564. CrossRef
    49. Neely, J. H. (1977). Semantic priming and retrieval from lexical memory: Roles of inhibitionless spreading activation and limited-capacity attention. / Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, / 106(3), 226-54. CrossRef
    50. Neely, J. H. (1991). Semantic priming effects in visual word recognition: A selective review of current findings and theories. In G. W. Humphreys & D. Besner (Eds.), / Basic processes in reading: Visual word recognition (pp. 264-37). New York: Routledge.
    51. Nelson, J. R., Balass, M., & Perfetti, C. A. (2005). Differences between written and spoken input in learning new words. / Written Language & Literacy, / 8(2), 25-4. doi:10.1075/wll.8.2.04nel . CrossRef
    52. Nelson, C., McDonnell, A. P., Johnston, S. S., Crompton, A., & Nelson, A. R. (2007). Keys to play: A strategy to increase the social interactions of young children with autism and their typically developing peers. / Education and Training in Developmental Disabilities, / 42, 165-81.
    53. O’Riordan, M., & Plaisted, K. (2001). Enhanced discrimination in autism. / Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology Section A—Human Experimental Psychology, / 54(4), 961-79. CrossRef
    54. Plaisted, K., O’Riordan, M., & Baron-Cohen, S. (1998). Enhanced visual search for a conjunctive target in autism: A research note. / Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, / 39(5), 777-83. CrossRef
    55. Plaisted, K., Saksida, L., Alcantara, J., & Weisblatt, E. (2003). Towards an understanding of the mechanisms of weak central coherence effects: Experiments in visual configural learning and auditory perception. / Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London Series B-Biological Sciences, / 358(1430), 375-86. doi:10.1098/rstb.2002.1211 . CrossRef
    56. Pratarelli, M. E., Perry, K. E., & Galloway, A. M. (1994). Automatic lexical access in children: New evidence from masked identity-priming. / Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, / 58(3), 346-58. doi:10.1006/jecp . 1994.1039. CrossRef
    57. Prior, M., Roberts, J., Rodgers, S., & Williams, K. (2011). / A review of the research to identify the most effective models of practice in early intervention for children with autism spectrum disorders. Canberra: Australian Government.
    58. Quill, K. (1997). Instructional considerations for young children with autism: The rationale for visually cued instruction. / Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, / 27(6), 697-14. doi:10.1023/A:1025806900162 . CrossRef
    59. Radeau, M. (1983). Semantic priming between spoken words in adults and children. / Canadian Journal of Psychology/Revue Canadienne de Psychologie, / 37(4), 547-56. doi:10.1037/h0080756 . CrossRef
    60. Rao, S., & Gagie, B. (2006). Learning through seeing and doing: Visual supports for children with autism. / Teaching Exceptional Children, / 38(6), 26-3.
    61. Rastle, K., Harrington, J., & Coltheart, M. (2002). 358, 534 nonwords: The ARC nonword database. / Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology Section A—Human Experimental Psychology, / 55, 1339-362. CrossRef
    62. Raven, J., Raven, J. C., & Court, J. H. (1998). / Coloured progressive matrices. (Vol. 2, Ravens Manual: Section?2). USA: Harcourt Assessment.
    63. Raven, J., Raven, J. C., & Court, J. H. (2000). / Standard progressive matrices (Ravens Manual: Section?3). USA: Pearson.
    64. Roberts, J. M. A., & Prior, M. (2006). / A review of the research to identify the most effective models of practice in early intervention for children with autism spectrum disorders. Canberra: Australian Government.
    65. Russo, N., Zecker, S., Trommer, B., Chen, J., & Kraus, N. (2009). Effects of background noise on cortical encoding of speech in autism spectrum disorders. / Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, / 39(8), 1185-196. doi:10.1007/s10803-009-0737-0 . CrossRef
    66. Schopler, E., & Mesibov, G. B. (1995). Introduction to learning and cognition in autism. In E. Schopler & G. B. Mesibov (Eds.), / Learning and cognition in autism (pp. 3-1). Plenum Press: New York. CrossRef
    67. Seiger-Gardner, L., & Schwartz, R. G. (2008). Lexical access in children with and without specific language impairment: A cross-modal picture-word interference study. / International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders, / 43(5), 528-51. doi:10.1080/13682820701768581 . CrossRef
    68. Semel, E., Wiig, E. H., & Secord, W. A. (2003). / The Clinical Evaluation of Language Fundamentals (4th ed.). San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corporation.
    69. Snedecor, G. W., & Cochran, W. G. (1989). / Statistical methods (8th ed.). Ames, IA: Iowa State University Press.
    70. Spears, C., & Turner, V. (2011). / Rising to new heights of communication and learning for children with autism: The definitive guide to using alternative-augmentative communication, visual strategies, and learning supports at home and school. London: Jessica Kingsley Publishers.
    71. Tabachnick, B. S., & Fidell, L. S. (2007). / Using multivariate statistics (5th ed.). Boston: Pearson Education.
    72. Taft, M. (2001). Cognitive psychology of lexical access. In N. J. Smelser & P. J. Baltes (Eds.), / International encyclopedia of the social & behavioral sciences (pp. 8743-748). Oxford: Pergamon. CrossRef
    73. Tager-Flusberg, H. (1985). Basic level and superordinate level categorization by autistic, mentally retarded, and normal children. / Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, / 40(3), 450-69. CrossRef
    74. Tager-Flusberg, H. (2006). Defining language phenotypes in autism. / Clinical Neuroscience Research, / 6(3-), 219-24. CrossRef
    75. Toichi, M., & Kamio, Y. (2001). Verbal association for simple common words in high-functioning autism. / Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, / 31(5), 483-90. doi:10.1023/A:1012216925216 . CrossRef
    76. Toichi, M., & Kamio, Y. (2002). Long-term memory and levels-of-processing in autism. / Neuropsychologia, / 40(7), 964-69. CrossRef
    77. Tse, C.-S., & Neely, J. H. (2007). Semantic priming from letter-searched primes occurs for low- but not high-frequency targets: Automatic semantic access may not be a myth. / Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, / 33(6), 1143-161. CrossRef
    78. van den Boer, M., de Jong, P. F., & Haentjens-van Meeteren, M. M. (2012). Lexical decision in children: Sublexical processing or lexical search? / Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, / 65(6), 1214-228. doi:10.1080/17470218.2011.652136 . CrossRef
    79. Van Overschelde, J. P., Rawson, K. A., & Dunlosky, J. (2004). Category norms: An updated and expanded version of the Battig and Montague (1969) norms. / Journal of Memory and Language, / 50(3), 289-35. doi:10.1016/j.jml.2003.10.003 . CrossRef
    80. Wang, S. H., Dong, X., Ren, Y. L., & Yang, Y. L. (2009). The development of semantic priming effect in childhood: An event-related potential study. / NeuroReport, / 20(6), 574-78. doi:10.1097/WNR.0b013e328329f215 . CrossRef
    81. Whitehouse, A. J. O., & Bishop, D. V. M. (2008). Cerebral dominance for language function in adults with specific language impairment or autism. / Brain, / 131, 3193-200. doi:10.1093/brain/awn266 . CrossRef
    82. Woodcock, R. W., Mather, N., & Shrank, F. A. (2004). / Woodcock-Johnson III diagnostic reading battery. Itasca, IL: Riverside Publishing.
  • 作者单位:Keely Harper-Hill (1) (2)
    David Copland (1) (2)
    Wendy Arnott (2)

    1. University of Queensland Centre for Clinical Research, Brisbane, Australia
    2. Division of Speech Pathology, School of Health and Rehabilitation, University of Queensland, Brisbane, Australia
  • ISSN:1573-3432
文摘
The provision of visual support to individuals with an autism spectrum disorder (ASD) is widely recommended. We explored one mechanism underlying the use of visual supports: efficiency of language processing. Two groups of children, one with and one without an ASD, participated. The groups had comparable oral and written language skills and nonverbal cognitive abilities. In two semantic priming experiments, prime modality and prime–target relatedness were manipulated. Response time and accuracy of lexical decisions on the spoken word targets were measured. In the first uni-modal experiment, both groups demonstrated significant priming effects. In the second experiment which was cross-modal, no effect for relatedness or group was found. This result is considered in the light of the attentional capacity required for access to the lexicon via written stimuli within the developing semantic system. These preliminary findings are also considered with respect to the use of visual support for children with ASD.

© 2004-2018 中国地质图书馆版权所有 京ICP备05064691号 京公网安备11010802017129号

地址:北京市海淀区学院路29号 邮编:100083

电话:办公室:(+86 10)66554848;文献借阅、咨询服务、科技查新:66554700