用户名: 密码: 验证码:
Attentional flexibility and memory capacity in conductors and pianists
详细信息    查看全文
  • 作者:Clemens Wöllner ; Andrea R. Halpern
  • 关键词:Selective and divided attention ; Working memory ; Long ; term memory ; Multimodal ; Expertise ; Cognitive aging
  • 刊名:Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics
  • 出版年:2016
  • 出版时间:January 2016
  • 年:2016
  • 卷:78
  • 期:1
  • 页码:198-208
  • 全文大小:552 KB
  • 参考文献:Alzahabi, R., & Becker, M. W. (2013). The association between media multitasking, task-switching, and dual-task performance. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 39, 1485–1495.PubMed
    Bailey, J. A., & Penhune, V. B. (2010). Rhythm synchronization performance and auditory working memory in early- and late-trained musicians. Experimental Brain Research, 204, 91–101.PubMed CrossRef
    Beilock, S. L., Carr, T. H., MacMahon, C., & Starkes, J. L. (2002). When paying attention becomes counterproductive: Impact of divided versus skill-focused attention on novice and experienced performance of sensorimotor skills. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 8, 6–16. doi:10.​1037/​1076-898X.​8.​1.​6 PubMed
    Benassi-Werke, M. E., Queiroz, M., Araújo, R. S., Bueno, O. F. A., & Oliveira, M. G. M. (2012). Musicians’ working memory for tones, words, and pseudowords. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 65, 1161–1171. doi:10.​1080/​17470218.​2011.​644799 CrossRef
    Berz, W. L. (1995). Working memory in music. A theoretical model. Music Perception, 12, 353–364.CrossRef
    Bigand, E., McAdams, S., & Forêt, S. (2000). Divided attention in music. International Journal of Psychology, 35, 270–278.CrossRef
    Bregman, A. S. (1990). Auditory scene analysis: The perceptual organization of sound. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
    Chaffin, R., Lisboa, T., Logan, T., & Begosh, K. T. (2010). Preparing for memorized cello performance: The role of performance cues. Psychology of Music, 38, 3–30.CrossRef
    Cherry, E. C. (1953). Some experiments on the recognition of speech, with one and with two ears. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 25, 975–979.CrossRef
    Colflesh, G. J. H., & Conway, A. R. A. (2007). Individual differences in working memory capacity and divided attention in dichotic listening. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 14, 699–703. doi:10.​3758/​BF03196824 CrossRef
    Conway, A. R. A., Cowan, N., & Bunting, M. F. (2001). The cocktail party phenomenon revisited: The importance of working memory capacity. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 8, 331–335. doi:10.​3758/​BF03196169 CrossRef
    Cowan, N. (1995). Attention and memory: An integrated framework. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.
    Daneman, M., & Carpenter, P. A. (1980). Individual differences in working memory and reading. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 19, 450–466. doi:10.​1016/​S0022-5371(80)90312-6 CrossRef
    Fritz, J. B., Elhilali, M., David, S. V., & Shamma, S. A. (2007). Auditory attention—focusing the searchlight on sound. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 17, 437–455.PubMed CrossRef
    Halpern, A. R., & Bartlett, J. C. (2002). Aging and memory for music: A review. Psychomusicology, 18, 10–27.CrossRef
    Hambrick, D. Z., & Engle, R. W. (2002). Effects of domain knowledge, working memory capacity, and age on cognitive performance: An investigation of the knowledge-is-power hypothesis. Cognitive Psychology, 44, 339–387.PubMed CrossRef
    Hansen, M., Wallentin, M., & Vuust, P. (2013). Working memory and musical competence of musicians and non-musicians. Psychology of Music, 41, 779–793.CrossRef
    Herzmann, G., & Curran, T. (2011). Experts’ memory: An ERP study of perceptual expertise effects on encoding and recognition. Memory & Cognition, 39, 412–432. doi:10.​3758/​s13421-010-0036-1 CrossRef
    Hodges, D. A., Hairston, W. D., & Burdette, J. H. (2006). Aspects of multisensory perception: The integration of visual and auditory information in musical experiences. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1060, 175–185.CrossRef
    Humes, L. E., Lee, J. H., & Coughlin, M. P. (2006). Auditory measures of selective and divided attention in young and older adults using single-talker competition. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 120, 2926–2937.PubMed CrossRef
    Johnson, J. A., & Zatorre, R. J. (2006). Neural substrates for dividing and focusing attention between simultaneous auditory and visual events. NeuroImage, 31, 1673–1681.PubMed CrossRef
    Keller, P. E. (2001). Attentional resource allocation in musical ensemble performance. Psychology of Music, 29, 20–38.CrossRef
    Lehnert, G., & Zimmer, H. D. (2006). Auditory and visual spatial working memory. Memory & Cognition, 34, 1080–1090. doi:10.​3758/​BF03193254 CrossRef
    Levitt, T., Fugelsang, J., & Crossley, M. (2006). Processing speed, attentional capacity, and age-related memory change. Experimental Aging Research, 32, 263–295.PubMed CrossRef
    Li, D., Cowan, N., & Saults, J. S. (2013). Estimating working memory capacity for lists of nonverbal sounds. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 75, 145–160. doi:10.​3758/​s13414-012-0383-z CrossRef
    Nager, W., Kohlmetz, C., Altenmüller, E., Rodriguez-Fornells, A., & Münte, T. F. (2003). The fate of sounds in conductors’ brains: An ERP study. Cognitive Brain Research, 17, 83–93. doi:10.​1016/​S0926-6410(03)00083-1 PubMed CrossRef
    Palmer, C. (2006). The nature of memory for music performance skills. In E. Altenmüller, M. Wiesendanger, & J. Kesselring (Eds.), Music, motor control and the brain (pp. 39–53). Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.CrossRef
    Parbery-Clark, A., Anderson, S., Hittner, E., & Kraus, N. (2012). Musical experience offsets age-related delays in neural timing. Neurobiology of Aging, 33(1483), e1–e4. doi:10.​1016/​j.​neurobiolaging.​2011.​12.​015 PubMed
    Parbery-Clark, A., Skoe, E., Lam, C., & Kraus, N. (2009). Musician enhancement for speech-in-noise. Ear and Hearing, 30, 653–661. doi:10.​1097/​AUD.​0b013e3181b412e9​ PubMed CrossRef
    Poudrier, È., & Repp, B. H. (2013). Can musicians track two beats simultaneously? Music Perception, 30, 369–390.CrossRef
    Ralph, B. C. W., Thomson, D. R., Seli, P., Carriere, J. S. A., & Smilek, D. (2015). Media multitasking and behavioral measures of sustained attention. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 77, 390–401. doi:10.​3758/​s13414-014-0771-7 CrossRef
    Rammsayer, T., & Altenmüller, E. (2006). Temporal information processing in musicians and nonmusicians. Music Perception, 24, 37–48.CrossRef
    Rammsayer, T. H., Buttkus, F., & Altenmüller, E. (2012). Musicians do better than non-musicians in both auditory and visual timing tasks. Music Perception, 30, 85–96.CrossRef
    Repp, B. H. (2010). Sensorimotor synchronization and perception of timing: Effects of music training and task experience. Human Movement Science, 29, 200–213.PubMed CrossRef
    Robert, C., Borella, E., Fagot, D., Lecerf, T., & de Ribaupierre, A. (2009). Working memory and inhibitory control across the life span: Intrusion errors in the Reading Span Test. Memory & Cognition, 37, 336–345.
    Ruggles, D. R., Freyman, R. L., & Oxenham, A. J. (2014). Influence of musical training on understanding voiced and whispered speech in noise. PLoS ONE, 9, e86980. doi:10.​1371/​journal.​pone.​0086980 PubMed PubMedCentral CrossRef
    Saults, J. S., & Cowan, N. (2007). A central capacity limit to the simultaneous storage of visual and auditory arrays in working memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 136, 663–684. doi:10.​1037/​0096-3445.​136.​4.​663 CrossRef
    Schendel, Z. A., & Palmer, C. (2007). Suppression effects on musical and verbal memory. Memory & Cognition, 35, 640–650.CrossRef
    Schlemmer, K. B., Kulke, F., Kuchinke, L., & Van der Meer, E. (2005). Absolute pitch and pupillary response: Effects of timbre and key color. Psychophysiology, 42, 465–472. doi:10.​1111/​j.​1469-8986.​2005.​00306.​x PubMed CrossRef
    Schulze, K., Zysset, S., Mueller, K., Friederici, A. D., & Koelsch, S. (2011). Neuroarchitecture of verbal and tonal working memory in nonmusicians and musicians. Human Brain Mapping, 32, 771–783. doi:10.​1002/​hbm.​21060 PubMed CrossRef
    Strait, D. L., Slater, J., O’Connell, S., & Kraus, N. (2015). Music training relates to the development of neural mechanisms of selective auditory attention. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience, 12, 94–104.PubMed CrossRef
    Swaminathan, J., Mason, C. R., Streeter, T. M., Best, V., Kidd, G., & Patel, A. D. (2015). Musical training, individual differences and the cocktail party problem. Scientific Reports, 5, 11628.PubMed PubMedCentral CrossRef
    White, E. J., Hutka, S. A., Williams, L. J., & Moreno, S. (2013). Learning, neural plasticity and sensitive periods: Implications for language acquisition, music training and transfer across the lifespan. Frontiers in Systems Neuroscience, 7, 90. doi:10.​3389/​fnsys.​2013.​00090 PubMed PubMedCentral CrossRef
    Williamson, V. J., Baddeley, A. D., & Hitch, G. J. (2010). Musicians’ and nonmusicians’ short-term memory for verbal and musical sequences: Comparing phonological similarity and pitch proximity. Memory & Cognition, 38, 163–175. doi:10.​3758/​MC.​38.​2.​163 CrossRef
    Wöllner, C. (2012). Self-recognition of highly skilled actions: A study of orchestral conductors. Consciousness and Cognition, 21, 1311–1321. doi:10.​1016/​j.​concog.​2012.​06.​006 PubMed CrossRef
    Wöllner, C., Deconinck, F. J. A., Parkinson, J., Hove, M. J., & Keller, P. E. (2012). The perception of prototypical motion: Synchronization is enhanced with quantitatively morphed gestures of musical conductors. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 38, 1390–1403. doi:10.​1037/​a0028130 PubMed
    Wöllner, C., & Williamon, A. (2007). An exploratory study of the role of performance feedback and musical imagery in piano playing. Research Studies in Music Education, 29, 39–54. doi:10.​1177/​1321103X07087567​ CrossRef
  • 作者单位:Clemens Wöllner (1)
    Andrea R. Halpern (2)

    1. Institute of Systematic Musicology, University of Hamburg, Hamburg, Germany
    2. Psychology Department, Bucknell University, Lewisburg, PA, USA
  • 刊物主题:Cognitive Psychology;
  • 出版者:Springer US
  • ISSN:1943-393X
文摘
Individuals with high working memory (WM) capacity also tend to have better selective and divided attention. Although both capacities are essential for skilled performance in many areas, evidence for potential training and expertise effects is scarce. We investigated the attentional flexibility of musical conductors by comparing them to equivalently trained pianists. Conductors must focus their attention both on individual instruments and on larger sections of different instruments. We studied students and professionals in both domains to assess the contributions of age and training to these skills. Participants completed WM span tests for auditory and visual (notated) pitches and timing durations, as well as long-term memory tests. In three dichotic attention tasks, they were asked to detect small pitch and timing deviations from two melodic streams presented in baseline (separate streams), selective-attention (concentrating on only one stream), and divided-attention (concentrating on targets in both streams simultaneously) conditions. Conductors were better than pianists in detecting timing deviations in divided attention, and experts detected more targets than students. We found no group differences for WM capacity or for pitch deviations in the attention tasks, even after controlling for the older age of the experts. Musicians’ WM spans across multimodal conditions were positively related to selective and divided attention. High-WM participants also had shorter reaction times in selective attention. Taken together, conductors showed higher attentional flexibility in successfully switching between different foci of attention.

© 2004-2018 中国地质图书馆版权所有 京ICP备05064691号 京公网安备11010802017129号

地址:北京市海淀区学院路29号 邮编:100083

电话:办公室:(+86 10)66554848;文献借阅、咨询服务、科技查新:66554700