用户名: 密码: 验证码:
A psycholinguistic database for traditional Chinese character naming
详细信息    查看全文
  • 作者:Ya-Ning Chang ; Chun-Hsien Hsu ; Jie-Li Tsai ; Chien-Liang Chen…
  • 关键词:Naming ; Chinese character naming ; Psycholinguistic norm ; Chinese visual word recognition
  • 刊名:Behavior Research Methods
  • 出版年:2016
  • 出版时间:March 2016
  • 年:2016
  • 卷:48
  • 期:1
  • 页码:112-122
  • 全文大小:361 KB
  • 参考文献:Aiken, L. S., & West, S. G. (1991). Multiple regression: Testing and interpreting interactions. New York: Sage.
    Balota, D. A., Cortese, M. J., Sergent-Marshall, S. D., Spieler, D. H., & Yap, M. J. (2004). Visual word recognition of single-syllable words. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 133, 283–316. doi:10.​1037/​0096-3445.​133.​2.​283 CrossRef
    Balota, D. A., Pilotti, M., & Cortese, M. J. (2001). Subjective frequency estimates for 2,938 monosyllabic words. Memory & Cognition, 29, 639–647. doi:10.​3758/​BF03200465 CrossRef
    Balota, D. A., Yap, M. J., Cortese, M. J., Hutchison, K. A., Kessler, B., Loftis, B., & Treiman, R. (2007). The English Lexicon Project. Behavior Research Methods, 39, 445–459. doi:10.​3758/​BF03193014 CrossRef PubMed
    Barca, L., Burani, C., & Arduino, L. S. (2002). Word naming times and psycholinguistic norms for italian nouns. Behavior Reseach Methods, Instruments, & Computers, 34, 424–434. doi:10.​3758/​BF03195471 CrossRef
    Chen, M. J., & Weekes, B. S. (2004). Effects of semantic radicals on chinese character categorization and character decision. Chinese Journal of Psychology, 46, 181–196.
    Coltheart, M. (1981). The MRC psycholinguistic database. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 33A, 497–505. doi:10.​1080/​1464074810840080​5 CrossRef
    Fang, S.-P., Horng, R.-Y., & Tzeng, O. J. L. (1986). Consistency effects in the chinese character and pseudo-character naming tasks. In H. S. R. Kao & R. Hoosain (Eds.), Linguistics, psychology, and the chinese language (pp. 11–21). Hong Kong: Center of Asian Studies, University of Hong Kong.
    Feldman, L. B., & Siok, W. W. T. (1999). Semantic radicals contribute to the visual identification of chinese characters. Journal of Memory and Language, 40, 559–576.CrossRef
    Ferrand, L., New, B., Brysbaert, M., Keuleers, E., Bonin, P., Méot, A., & Pallier, C. (2010). The French Lexicon Project: Lexical decision data for 38,840 French words and 38,840 pseudowords. Behavior Research Methods, 42, 488–496. doi:10.​3758/​BRM.​42.​2.​488 CrossRef PubMed
    Glushko, R. J. (1979). The organization and activation of orthographic knowledge in reading aloud. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 5, 674–691. doi:10.​1037/​0096-1523.​5.​4.​674
    Hair, J. F., Black, W. C., Babin, B. J., & Anderson, R. E. (2009). Multivariate data analysis (7th ed.). New York, NY: Prentice Hall.
    Hsu, C.-H., Lee, C.-Y., & Marantz, A. (2011). Effects of visual complexity and sublexical information in the occipitotemporal cortex in the reading of Chinese phonograms: A single-trial analysis with meg. Brain and Language, 117, 1–11.CrossRef PubMed
    Hsu, C.-H., Tsai, J.-L., Lee, C.-Y., & Tzeng, O. J. (2009). Orthographic combinability and phonological consistency effects in reading chinese phonograms: An event-related potential study. Brain and Language, 108, 56–66.CrossRef PubMed
    Huang, C.-R., & Chen, K.-J. (1998). Academia Sinica balanced corpus. Retrieved from www.​sinica.​edu.​tw/​SinicaCorpus/​
    Huang, C.-Y., Lee, C. Y., Huang, H.-W., & Chou, C.-J. (2011). Number of sense effects of chinese disyllabic compounds in the two hemispheres. Brain and Language, 119, 99–109.CrossRef PubMed
    Huang, H.-W., Lee, C. Y., Tsai, J. L., Lee, C. L., Hung, D. L., & Tzeng, O. J. (2006). Orthographic neighborhood effects in reading chinese two-character words. NeuroReport, 17, 1061–1065.CrossRef PubMed
    Hue, C.-W. (1992). Recognition processes in character naming. In H.-C. E. Chen & O. J. L. Tzeng (Eds.), Language processing in Chinese (Advances in Psychology (Vol. 90, pp. 93–107). Amsterdam, The Netherlands: North-Holland.CrossRef
    Jared, D. (1997). Spelling–sound consistency affects the naming of high-frequency words. Journal of Memory and Language, 36, 505–529.CrossRef
    Jared, D., McRae, K., & Seidenberg, M. S. (1990). The basis of consistency effects in word naming. Journal of Memory and Language, 29, 687–715.CrossRef
    Keuleers, E., Diependaele, K., & Brysbaert, M. (2010). Practice effects in large-scale visual word recognition studies: A lexical decision study on 14,000 Dutch mono- and disyllabic words and nonwords. Frontiers in Psychology, 1(174), 1–15. doi:10.​3389/​fpsyg.​2010.​00174
    Keuleers, E., Lacey, P., Rastle, K., & Brysbaert, M. (2012). The British Lexicon Project: Lexical decision data for 28,730 monosyllabic and disyllabic English words. Behavior Research Methods, 44, 287–304. doi:10.​3758/​s13428-011-0118-4 CrossRef PubMed PubMedCentral
    Lee, C.-Y., Huang, H.-W., Kuo, W.-J., Tsai, J.-L., & Tzeng, J.-L. O. (2010). Cognitive and neural basis of the consistency and lexicality effects in reading Chinese. Journal of Neurolinguistics, 23, 10–27. doi:10.​1016/​j.​jneuroling.​2009.​07.​003 CrossRef
    Lee, C.-Y., Tsai, J. L., Kuo, W. J., Yeh, T. C., Wu, Y. T., Ho, L. T., & Hsieh, J. C. (2004). Neuronal correlates of consistency and frequency effects on chinese character naming: An event-related fMRI study. NeuroImage, 23, 1235–1245.CrossRef PubMed
    Lee, C.-Y., Tsai, J.-L., Su, E. C.-I., Tzeng, O. J., & Hung, D. L. (2005). Consistency, regularity, and frequency effects in naming chinese characters. Language and Linguistics, 6, 75–107.
    Leong, C. K., Cheng, P.-W., & Mulcahy, R. (1987). Automatic processing of morphemic orthography by mature readers. Language and Speech, 30, 181–196.PubMed
    Li, Q.-L., Bi, H.-Y., Wei, T.-Q., & Chen, B.-G. (2011). Orthographic neighborhood size effect in Chinese character naming: Orthographic and phonological activations. Acta Psychologica, 136, 35–41.CrossRef PubMed
    Liu, Y., Shu, H., & Li, P. (2007). Word naming and psycholinguistic norms: Chinese. Behavior Research Methods, 39, 192–198. doi:10.​3758/​BF03193147 CrossRef PubMed
    McBride-Chang, C., Chow, B. W. Y., Zhong, Y., Burgess, S., & Hayward, W. G. (2005). Chinese character acquisition and visual skills in two Chinese scripts. Reading and Writing, 18, 99–128.CrossRef
    Ministry of Education, Taiwan. (2012). Table of Mandarin polyphon. Taiwan: Mandarin Promotion Council, Ministry of Education, Taiwan. Retrieved from www.​edu.​tw/​pages/​detail.​aspx?​Node=​3692&​Page=​16373&​WID=​c5ad5187-55ef-4811-8219-e946fe04f725
    Murray, W. S., & Forster, K. I. (2004). Serial mechanisms in lexical access: The rank hypothesis. Psychological Review, 111, 721–756. doi:10.​1037/​0033-295X.​111.​3.​721 CrossRef PubMed
    New, B., Ferrand, L., Pallier, C., & Brysbaert, M. (2006). Reexamining the word length effect in visual word recognition: New evidence from the english lexicon project. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 13, 45–52. doi:10.​3758/​BF03193811 CrossRef
    Perfetti, C. A., & Tan, L. H. (1998). The time course of graphic, phonological, and semantic activation in Chinese character identification. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 24, 101–118. doi:10.​1037/​0278-7393.​24.​1.​101
    Perry, C., Ziegler, J. C., & Zorzi, M. (2007). Nested incremental modeling in the development of computational theories: The CDP+ model of reading aloud. Psychological Review, 114, 273–315. doi:10.​1037/​0033-295X.​114.​2.​273 CrossRef PubMed
    Plaut, D. C., McClelland, J. L., Seidenberg, M. S., & Patterson, K. (1996). Understanding normal and impaired word reading: Computational principles in quasi-regular domains. Psychological Review, 103, 56–115. doi:10.​1037/​0033-295X.​103.​1.​56 CrossRef PubMed
    Rodd, J., Gaskell, G., & Marslen-Wilson, W. (2002). Making sense of semantic ambiguity: Semantic competition in lexical access. Journal of Memory & Language, 46, 245–266. doi:10.​1006/​jmla.​2001.​2810 CrossRef
    Seidenberg, M. S., & McClelland, J. L. (1989). A distributed, developmental model of word recognition and naming. Psychological Review, 96, 523–568. doi:10.​1037/​0033-295X.​96.​4.​523 CrossRef PubMed
    Su, Y.-F., & Samuels, S. J. (2010). Developmental changes in character-complexity and word-length effects when reading Chinese script. Reading and Writing, 23, 1085–1108.CrossRef
    Sze, W. P., Rickard Liow, S. J., & Yap, M. J. (2013). The Chinese Lexicon Project: A repository of lexical decision behavioral responses for 2,500 Chinese characters. Behavior Research Methods, 46, 263–273. doi:10.​3758/​s13428-013-0355-9 CrossRef
    Tan, L. H., Hoosain, R., & Siok, W. W. T. (1996). Activation of phonological codes before access to character meaning in written Chinese. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 22, 865–882. doi:10.​1037/​0278-7393.​22.​4.​865
    Taraban, R., & McClelland, J. L. (1987). Conspiracy effects in word pronunciation. Journal of Memory and Language, 26, 608–631.CrossRef
    Tsai, J.-L., Lee, C. Y., Lin, Y. C., Tzeng, O. J., & Hung, D. L. (2006). Neighborhood size effects of chinese words in lexical decision and reading. Language and Linguistics, 7, 659–675.
    Woollams, A. M. (2005). Imageability and ambiguity effects in speeded naming: Convergence and divergence. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 31, 878–890. doi:10.​1037/​0278-7393.​31.​5.​878 PubMed
    Yang, J., McCandliss, B. D., Shu, H., & Zevin, J. D. (2009). Simulating language-specific and language-general effects in a statistical learning model of chinese reading. Journal of Memory and Language, 61, 238–257.CrossRef PubMed PubMedCentral
    Yap, M. J., Rickard Liow, S. J., Jalil, S. B., & Faizal, S. S. B. (2010). The Malay Lexicon Project: A database of lexical statistics for 9,592 words. Behavior Research Methods, 42, 992–1003. doi:10.​3758/​BRM.​42.​4.​992 CrossRef PubMed
    Zhou, Y. G. (1978). To what degree are the “phonetics” of present day Chinese characters still phonetic? Zhongguo Yuwen, 146, 172–177.
  • 作者单位:Ya-Ning Chang (1)
    Chun-Hsien Hsu (1)
    Jie-Li Tsai (2) (5)
    Chien-Liang Chen (1)
    Chia-Ying Lee (1) (3) (4)

    1. Institute of Linguistics, Academia Sinica, Taipei City, Taiwan
    2. Department of Psychology, National Chengchi University, Taipei City, Taiwan
    5. Research Center for Mind, Brain and Learning, National Chengchi University, Taipei City, Taiwan
    3. Institute of Neuroscience, National Yang-Ming University, Taipei City, Taiwan
    4. Institute of Cognitive Neuroscience, National Central University, Taoyuan City, Taiwan
  • 刊物主题:Cognitive Psychology;
  • 出版者:Springer US
  • ISSN:1554-3528
文摘
In this study, we aimed to provide a large-scale set of psycholinguistic norms for 3,314 traditional Chinese characters, along with their naming reaction times (RTs), collected from 140 Chinese speakers. The lexical and semantic variables in the database include frequency, regularity, familiarity, consistency, number of strokes, homophone density, semantic ambiguity rating, phonetic combinability, semantic combinability, and the number of disyllabic compound words formed by a character. Multiple regression analyses were conducted to examine the predictive powers of these variables for the naming RTs. The results demonstrated that these variables could account for a significant portion of variance (55.8 %) in the naming RTs. An additional multiple regression analysis was conducted to demonstrate the effects of consistency and character frequency. Overall, the regression results were consistent with the findings of previous studies on Chinese character naming. This database should be useful for research into Chinese language processing, Chinese education, or cross-linguistic comparisons. The database can be accessed via an online inquiry system (http://​ball.​ling.​sinica.​edu.​tw/​namingdatabase/​index.​html).

© 2004-2018 中国地质图书馆版权所有 京ICP备05064691号 京公网安备11010802017129号

地址:北京市海淀区学院路29号 邮编:100083

电话:办公室:(+86 10)66554848;文献借阅、咨询服务、科技查新:66554700