用户名: 密码: 验证码:
No need for a social cue! A masked magician can also trick the audience in the vanishing ball illusion
详细信息    查看全文
  • 作者:Cyril Thomas ; André Didierjean
  • 关键词:Magic ; Social cueing ; Perception ; Illusion
  • 刊名:Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics
  • 出版年:2016
  • 出版时间:January 2016
  • 年:2016
  • 卷:78
  • 期:1
  • 页码:21-29
  • 全文大小:1,164 KB
  • 参考文献:Cui, J., Otero-Millan, J., Macknik, S. L., King, M., & Martinez-Conde, S. (2011). Social misdirection fails to enhance a magic illusion. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 5, 103. doi:10.​3389/​fnhum.​2011.​00103 PubMed PubMedCentral CrossRef
    Dalton, K. M., Nacewicz, B. M., Johnstone, T., Schaefer, H. S., Gernsbacher, M. A., Goldsmith, H. H., … Davidson, R. J. (2005). Gaze fixation and the neural circuitry of face processing in autism. Nature Neuroscience, 8, 519–526.
    Didierjean, A., Ferrari, V., & Blättler, C. (2014). Role of knowledge in motion extrapolation: The relevance of an approach contrasting experts and novices. In B. H. Ross (Ed.), The psychology of learning and motivation (Vol. 61, pp. 215–235). San Diego: Academic Press.
    Driver, J., Davis, G., Kidd, P., Maxwell, E., Ricciardelli, P., & Baron-Cohen, S. (1999). Gaze perception triggers reflexive visuospatial orienting. Visual Cognition, 6, 509–541.CrossRef
    Freyd, J. J., & Finke, R. A. (1984). Representational momentum. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 10, 126–132. doi:10.​1037/​0278-7393.​10.​1.​126
    Hegdé, J., Fang, F., Murray, S. O., & Kersten, D. (2008). Preferential responses to occluded objects in the human visual cortex. Journal of Vision, 8(4), 16.1–16. doi:10.​1167/​8.​4.​16 CrossRef
    Howard, C. J., & Holcombe, A. O. (2010). Unexpected changes in direction of motion attract attention. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 72, 2087–2095. doi:10.​3758/​BF03196685 CrossRef
    Hubbard, T. L. (2005). Representational momentum and related displacements in spatial memory: A review of the findings. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 12, 822–851. doi:10.​3758/​BF03196775 CrossRef
    Hubbard, T. L. (2015). The varieties of momentum-like experience. Psychological Bulletin, 141, 1081–1119. doi:10.​1037/​bul0000016 PubMed CrossRef
    Hubbard, T. L., & Bharucha, J. J. (1988). Judged displacement in apparent vertical and horizontal motion. Perception & Psychophysics, 44, 211–221. doi:10.​3758/​BF03206290 CrossRef
    Hubbard, T. L., & Motes, M. A. (2005). An effect of context on whether memory for initial position exhibits a Fröhlich effect or an onset repulsion effect. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 58, 961–979.PubMed CrossRef
    Kanizsa, G., & Gerbino, W. (1982). Amodal completion: Seeing or thinking? In J. Beck (Ed.), Organization and representation in perception (pp. 167–190). Hillsdale: Erlbaum.
    Kuhn, G., Caffaratti, H. A., Teszka, R., & Rensink, R. A. (2014). A psychologically-based taxonomy of misdirection. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 1392. doi:10.​3389/​fpsyg.​2014.​01392 PubMed PubMedCentral CrossRef
    Kuhn, G., Kourkoulou, A., & Leekam, S. R. (2010). How magic changes our expectations about autism. Psychological Science, 21, 1487–1493. doi:10.​1177/​0956797610383435​ PubMed CrossRef
    Kuhn, G., & Land, M. F. (2006). There’s more to magic than meets the eye. Current Biology, 16, R950–R951. doi:10.​1016/​j.​cub.​2006.​10.​012 PubMed CrossRef
    Kuhn, G., & Rensink, A. R. (2015). The vanishing ball illusion:  A new perspective on the perception of dynamic events. Cognition (in press)
    Kuhn, G., Tatler, B. W., & Cole, G. G. (2009). You look where I look! Effect of gaze cues on overt and covert attention in misdirection. Visual Cognition, 17, 925–944. doi:10.​1080/​1350628090282677​5 CrossRef
    Kuhn, G., Teszka, R., Tenaw, N., & Kingstone, A. (2015). Don’t be fooled! Attentional responses to social cues in a face-to-face and video magic trick reveals greater top-down control for overt than covert attention. Cognition, 146, 136–142. doi:10.​1016/​j.​cognition.​2015.​08.​005 PubMed CrossRef
    Mansfield, E., Farroni, T., & Johnson, M. H. (2003). Does gaze perception facilitate overt orienting? Visual Cognition, 10, 7–14.CrossRef
    Michotte, A., Thines, G., & Crabbe, G. (1991). Amodal completion of perceptual structures. In G. Thines, A. Costall, & G. Butterworth (Eds.), Michotte’s experimental phenomenology of perception (pp. 140–167). Hillsdale: Erlbaum (Original work published 1964).
    Pelphrey, K. A., Sasson, N. J., Reznick, J. S., Paul, G., Goldman, B. D., & Piven, J. (2002). Visual scanning of faces in autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 32, 249–261.PubMed CrossRef
    Rensink, R. A., & Kuhn, G. (2015). The possibility of a science of magic. Frontiers in Psychology, 6, 1576. doi:10.​3389/​fpsyg.​2015.​01576 PubMed PubMedCentral CrossRef
    Riby, D. M., & Hancock, P. J. (2008). Viewing it differently: Social scene perception in Williams syndrome and autism. Neuropsychologia, 46, 2855–2860.PubMed CrossRef
    Sasson, N., Tsuchiya, N., Hurley, R., Couture, S. M., Penn, D. L., Adolphs, R., & Piven, J. (2007). Orienting to social stimuli differentiates social cognitive impairment in autism and schizophrenia. Neuropsychologia, 45, 2580–2588. doi:10.​1016/​j.​neuropsychologia​.​2007.​03.​009 PubMed PubMedCentral CrossRef
    Tachibana, R. (2014). The effects of social misdirection on magic tricks: How deceived and undeceived groups differ. i-Perception, 5, 143–146.PubMed PubMedCentral CrossRef
    Tatler, B. W., & Kuhn, G. (2007). Don’t look now: The magic of misdirection. In R. van Gompel, M. Fischer, W. Murray, & R. L. Hill (Eds.), Eye movement research: Insights into mind and brain (pp. 697–714). Amsterdam: Elsevier.CrossRef
    Thomas, C., Didierjean, A., Maquestiaux, F., & Gygax, P. (2015). Does magic offer a cryptozoology ground for psychology? Review of General Psychology, 19, 117–128.CrossRef
    Triplett, N. (1900). The psychology of conjuring deceptions. American Journal of Psychology, 11, 439–510.CrossRef
    Tse, P. U. (1999). Volume completion. Cognitive Psychology, 39, 37–68. doi:10.​1006/​cogp.​1999.​0715 PubMed CrossRef
    van Lier, R., & Wagemans, J. (1999). From images to objects: Global and local completions of self-occluded parts. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 25, 1721–1741. doi:10.​1037/​0096-1523.​25.​6.​1721
  • 作者单位:Cyril Thomas (1)
    André Didierjean (1)

    1. University of Franche-Comté, 30 rue Mégevand, 25030, Besançon, France
  • 刊物主题:Cognitive Psychology;
  • 出版者:Springer US
  • ISSN:1943-393X
文摘
In the vanishing ball illusion (VBI), a magician throws a ball up in the air twice, after which he pretends to toss it up again, when in fact it remains secretly concealed in his hand. Observers perceive an imaginary ball disappearing into the air. According to Kuhn and Land (2006), the VBI during the fake throw is mediated by the magician’s gaze and/or head direction (also called “social cues”) as he looks toward the imaginary ball. The aim of this article is to test an alternative interpretation. According to our hypothesis, the magician’s social cues are not essential to the VBI. We compared the numbers of participants experiencing the VBI when the magician’s social cues were directed toward the illusory ball and when the magician’s social cues were either hidden behind a black mask (Exp. 1) or stationary (Exp. 2). The results showed that the number of observers experiencing the VBI was high (almost two-thirds of the participants), regardless of whether the magician’s social cueing was directed toward the illusion, hidden behind a mask, or stationary. In a third experiment (Exp. 3), we replicated Kuhn and Land’s initial results and attempted to further explain their “anti-illusion” social-cue effect. This study confirms that social cueing is not required in the VBI: Its presence did not increase the number of participants experiencing the illusion.

© 2004-2018 中国地质图书馆版权所有 京ICP备05064691号 京公网安备11010802017129号

地址:北京市海淀区学院路29号 邮编:100083

电话:办公室:(+86 10)66554848;文献借阅、咨询服务、科技查新:66554700