用户名: 密码: 验证码:
Atypical Neural Activity in Males But Not Females with Autism Spectrum Disorder
详细信息    查看全文
  • 作者:Melissa Kirkovski ; Peter G. Enticott…
  • 关键词:Autism spectrum disorder ; Theory of mind ; Mentalizing network ; Sex differences ; Medial prefrontal cortex ; Temporo ; parietal junction ; Superior temporal sulcus ; Functional magnetic resonance imaging
  • 刊名:Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders
  • 出版年:2016
  • 出版时间:March 2016
  • 年:2016
  • 卷:46
  • 期:3
  • 页码:954-963
  • 全文大小:795 KB
  • 参考文献:Abell, F., Happe, F., & Frith, U. (2000). Do triangles play tricks? Attribution of mental states to animated shapes in normal and abnormal development. Cognitive Development, 15, 1–16.CrossRef
    Alaerts, K., Woolley, D. G., Steyaert, J., Di Martino, A., Swinnen, S. P., & Wenderoth, N. (2014). Underconnectivity of the superior temporal sulcus predicts emotion recognition deficits in autism. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 9(10), 1589–1600.
    American Psychiatric Association. (2013). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (5th ed.). Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Publishing.
    Attwood, T. (2007). The complete guide to asperger’s syndrome. London: Jessica Kingsley Publishers.
    Austin, E. J. (2005). Personality correlates of the broader autism phenotype as assessed by the Autism Spectrum Quotient (AQ). Personality and Individual Differences, 38, 451–460.CrossRef
    Baron-Cohen, S., Leslie, A. M., & Frith, U. (1985). Does the autistic child have a “theory of mind”? Cognition, 21, 37–46.CrossRef PubMed
    Baron-Cohen, S., & Wheelwright, S. (2004). The empathy quotient: An investigation of adults with Asperger syndrome or high functioning autism, and normal sex differences. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 34, 163–175.CrossRef PubMed
    Baron-Cohen, S., Wheelwright, S. J., Skinner, R., Martin, J., & Clubley, E. (2001). The autism-spectrum quotient (AQ): Evidence of Asperger’s syndrome/high functioning autism, males and females, scientists and mathematicians. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 31, 5–17.CrossRef PubMed
    Beacher, F., et al. (2012). Autism attenuates sex differences in brain structure: A combined voxel-based morphometry and diffusion tensor imaging study. American Journal of Neuroradiology, 33, 83–89.CrossRef PubMed
    Bloss, C. S., & Courchesne, E. (2007). MRI neuroanatomy in young girls with autism: A preliminary study. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 46, 515–523.CrossRef PubMed
    Brett, M., Anton, J.-L., Valabregue, R., & Poline, J.-B. (2002). Region of interest analysis using the MarsBar toolbox for SPM 99. Neuroimage, 16, S497.
    Castelli, F., Frith, C., Happe, F., & Frith, U. (2002). Autism, Asperger syndrome and brain mechanisms for the attribution of mental states to animated shapes. Brain, 125, 1839–1849.CrossRef PubMed
    Castelli, F., Happe, F., Frith, U., & Frith, C. (2000). Movement and mind: A functional imaging study of perception and interpretation of complex intentional movement patterns. NeuroImage, 12, 314–325.CrossRef PubMed
    CDC. (2014). Prevalence of autism spectrum disorders among children aged 8 years—Autism and developmental diabilities monitoring network, 11 sites, United states, 2010. Morbidity and Mortality Weekly Report, 63, 1–22.
    Dufour, N., et al. (2013). Similar brain activation during false belief tasks in a large sample of adults with and without autism. PLoS One, 8, e75468.PubMedCentral CrossRef PubMed
    Falkmer, T., Anderson, K., Falkmer, M., & Horlin, C. (2013). Diagnostic procedures in autism spectrum disorders: A systematic literature review. European Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 22, 329–340.CrossRef PubMed
    Fein, D., et al. (2013). Optimal outcome in individuals with a history of autism. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 54, 195–205.PubMedCentral CrossRef PubMed
    Fombonne, E. (2003). Epidemiological surveys of autsim and other prevasive developmental disorders: An update. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 33, 365–382.CrossRef PubMed
    Fombonne, E. (2009). Epidemiology of pervasive developmental disorders. Pediatric Research, 65, 591–598.CrossRef PubMed
    Frith, C. D., & Frith, U. (1999). Interacting minds–a biological basis. Science, 286, 1692–1695.CrossRef PubMed
    Gallagher, H. L., & Frith, C. D. (2003). Functional imaging of ‘theory of mind’. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 7, 77–83.CrossRef PubMed
    Gotham, K., Pickles, A., & Lord, C. (2012). Trajectories of autism severity in children using standardized ADOS scores. Pediatrics, 130, e1278–e1284.PubMedCentral CrossRef PubMed
    Holt, R., et al. (2014). ‘Reading the Mind in the Eyes’: An fMRI study of adolescents with autism and their siblings. Psychological Medicine, 44, 3215–3227.CrossRef PubMed
    Kana, R. K., Libero, L. E., Hu, C. P., Deshpande, H. D., & Colburn, J. S. (2014). Functional brain networks and white matter underlying theory-of-mind in autism. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 9, 98–105.PubMedCentral CrossRef PubMed
    Kirkovski, M., Enticott, P. G., & Fitzgerald, P. B. (2013). A review of the role of female gender in autism spectrum disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 43, 2584–2603. doi:10.​1007/​s10803-013-1811-1 .CrossRef PubMed
    Kirkovski, M., Enticott, P. G., Maller, J. J., Rossell, S. L., & Fitzgerald, P. B. (2015). Diffusion tensor imaging reveals no white matter impairments among adults with autism spectrum disorder. Psychiatry Research: Neuroimaging. doi:10.​1016/​j.​pscychresns.​2015.​05.​003
    Kopp, S., & Gillberg, C. (2011). The Autism Spectrum Screening Questionnaire (ASSQ)-Revised Extended Version (ASSQ-REV): An instrument for better capturing the autism phenotype in girls? A preliminary study involving 191 clinical cases and community controls. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 32, 2875–2888.CrossRef PubMed
    Lai, M.-C., et al. (2013). Biological sex affects the neurobiology of autism. Brain, 136, 2799–2815.PubMedCentral CrossRef PubMed
    Lee, S. M., Gao, T., & McCarthy, G. (2014). Attributing intentions to random motion engages the posterior superior temporal sulcus. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 9, 81–87.PubMedCentral CrossRef PubMed
    Lombardo, M. V., Chakrabarti, B., Bullmore, E. T., & Baron-Cohen, S. (2011). Specialization of right temporo-parietal junction for mentalizing and its relation to social impairments in autism. NeuroImage, 56, 1832–1838.CrossRef PubMed
    Lovén, J., Svärd, J., Ebner, N. C., Herlitz, A., & Fischer, H. (2013). Face gender modulates women’s brain activity during face encoding. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 9, 1000–1005.PubMedCentral CrossRef PubMed
    Mainieri, A. G., Heim, S., Straube, B., Binkofski, F., & Kircher, T. (2013). Differential role of the Mentalizing and the Mirror Neuron system in the imitation of communicative gestures. NeuroImage, 81, 294–305.CrossRef PubMed
    Mathersul, D., McDonald, S., & Rushby, J. A. (2013). Understanding advanced theory of mind and empathy in high-functioning adults with autism spectrum disorder. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 35, 655–668.CrossRef PubMed
    Matson, J. L., & Williams, L. W. (2014). Depression and mood disorders among persons with Autism Spectrum Disorders. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 35, 2003–2007.CrossRef PubMed
    McLennan, J. D., Lord, C., & Schopler, E. (1993). Sex differences in higher functioning people with autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 23, 217–227.CrossRef PubMed
    Oldfield, R. C. (1971). The assessment and analysis of handedness: The Edinburgh inventory. Neuropsychologia, 9, 97–113.CrossRef PubMed
    Philip, R., Dauvermann, M. R., Whalley, H. C., Baynham, K., Lawrie, S. M., & Stanfield, A. C. (2012). A systematic review and meta-analysis of the fMRI investigation of autism spectrum disorders. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 36, 901–942.CrossRef PubMed
    Rajendran, G., & Mitchell, P. (2007). Cognitive theories of autism. Developmental Review, 27, 224–260.CrossRef
    Redcay, E., & Courchesne, E. (2005). When is the brain enlarged in autism? A meta-analysis of all brain size reports. Biological Psychiatry, 58, 1–9.CrossRef PubMed
    Ritvo, R. A., et al. (2011). The Ritvo Autism Asperger diagnostic scale-revised (RAADS-R): A scale to assist the diagnosis of autism spectrum disorder in adults—An international validation study. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 41, 1076–1089.PubMedCentral CrossRef PubMed
    Saxe, R., & Powell, L. J. (2006). It’s the thought that counts specific brain regions for one component of theory of mind. Psychological Science, 17, 692–699.CrossRef PubMed
    Saxe, R., Xiao, D.-K., Kovacs, G., Perrett, D., & Kanwisher, N. (2004). A region of right posterior superior temporal sulcus responds to observed intentional actions. Neuropsychologia, 42, 1435–1446.CrossRef PubMed
    Schaafsma, S. M., Pfaff, D. W., Spunt, R. P., & Adolphs, R. (2015). Deconstructing and reconstructing theory of mind. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 19, 65–72.PubMedCentral CrossRef PubMed
    Scheeren, A. M., de Rosnay, M., Koot, H. M., & Begeer, S. (2013). Rethinking theory of mind in high-functioning autism spectrum disorder. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 54, 628–635.CrossRef PubMed
    Schumann, C. M., et al. (2009). Amygdala enlargement in toddlers with autism related to severity of social and communication impairments. Biological Psychiatry, 66, 942–949.PubMedCentral CrossRef PubMed
    Schumann, C. M., et al. (2010). Longitudinal magnetic resonance imaging study of cortical development through early childhood in autism. The Journal of Neuroscience, 30, 4419–4427.PubMedCentral CrossRef PubMed
    Schurz, M., Radua, J., Aichhorn, M., Richlan, F., & Perner, J. (2014). Fractionating theory of mind: A meta-analysis of functional brain imaging studies. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 42, 9–34.
    Silani, G., Bird, G., Brindley, R., Singer, T., Frith, C., & Frith, U. (2008). Levels of emotional awareness and autism: An fMRI study. Social Neuroscience, 3, 97–112.CrossRef PubMed
    Stoesz, B. M., Montgomery, J. M., Smart, S. L., & Hellsten, L.-A. M. (2011). Review of five instruments for the assessment of Asperger’s disorder in adults. The Clinical Neuropsychologist, 25, 376–401.CrossRef PubMed
    Van Overwalle, F. (2011). A dissociation between social mentalizing and general reasoning. Neuroimage, 54, 1589–1599.CrossRef PubMed
    Van Overwalle, F., & Baetens, K. (2009). Understanding others’ actions and goals by mirror and mentalizing systems: A meta-analysis. Neuroimage, 48, 564–584.CrossRef PubMed
    Wang, A. T., Lee, S. S., Sigman, M., & Dapretto, M. (2007). Reading affect in the face and voice: Neural correlates of interpreting communicative intent in children and adolescents with autism spectrum disorders. Archives of General Psychiatry, 64, 698–708.PubMedCentral CrossRef PubMed
    Wass, S. (2011). Distortions and disconnections: Disrupted brain connectivity in autism. Brain and Cognition, 75, 18–28.CrossRef PubMed
    White, S. J., Coniston, D., Rogers, R., & Frith, U. (2011). Developing the Frith Happé animations: A quick and objective test of Theory of Mind for adults with autism. Autism Research, 4, 149–154.CrossRef PubMed
    White, S., Frith, U., Rellecke, J., Al-Noor, Z., & Gilbert, S. J. (2014). Autistic adolescents show atypical activation of the brain’s mentalizing system even without a prior history of mentalizing problems. Neuropsychologia, 56, 17–25.CrossRef PubMed
    Zilbovicius, M., Meresse, I., Chabane, N., Brunelle, F., Samson, Y., & Boddaert, N. (2006). Autism, the superior temporal sulcus and social perception. Trends in Neurosciences, 29, 359–366.CrossRef PubMed
    Zilbovicius, M., et al. (2013). Autism, social cognition and superior temporal sulcus. Open Journal of Psychiatry, 3, 46–55.CrossRef
  • 作者单位:Melissa Kirkovski (1) (2)
    Peter G. Enticott (1) (2)
    Matthew E. Hughes (3)
    Susan L. Rossell (1) (3)
    Paul B. Fitzgerald (1)

    1. Monash Alfred Psychiatry Research Centre, The Alfred and Central Clinical School, Monash University, Melbourne, VIC, Australia
    2. Cognitive Neuroscience Unit, School of Psychology, Deakin University, Melbourne Burwood Campus, Building BC, Level 5, 221 Burwood Hwy, Burwood, VIC, 3125, Australia
    3. Brain and Psychological Science Research Centre, Faculty Health, Arts and Design, Swinburne University, Hawthorn, VIC, Australia
  • 刊物类别:Behavioral Science
  • 刊物主题:Psychology
    Child and School Psychology
    Pediatrics
  • 出版者:Springer Netherlands
  • ISSN:1573-3432
文摘
The medial prefrontal cortex (mPFC) and the right temporo-parietal junction (rTPj) are highly involved in social understanding, a core area of impairment in autism spectrum disorder (ASD). We used fMRI to investigate sex differences in the neural correlates of social understanding in 27 high-functioning adults with ASD and 23 matched controls. There were no differences in neural activity in the mPFC or rTPj between groups during social processing. Whole brain analysis revealed decreased activity in the posterior superior temporal sulcus in males with ASD compared to control males while processing social information. This pattern was not observed in the female sub-sample. The current study indicates that sex mediates the neurobiology of ASD, particularly with respect to processing social information. Keywords Autism spectrum disorder Theory of mind Mentalizing network Sex differences Medial prefrontal cortex Temporo-parietal junction Superior temporal sulcus Functional magnetic resonance imaging

© 2004-2018 中国地质图书馆版权所有 京ICP备05064691号 京公网安备11010802017129号

地址:北京市海淀区学院路29号 邮编:100083

电话:办公室:(+86 10)66554848;文献借阅、咨询服务、科技查新:66554700