用户名: 密码: 验证码:
海洛因戒治者隐藏信息的ERP研究
详细信息    本馆镜像全文|  推荐本文 |  |   获取CNKI官网全文
摘要
说谎是常见的社会现象,涉及到诸如注意、记忆、执行控制等高级认知加工功能。海洛因成瘾者历来都被认为是习惯性说谎甚至是病理性说谎的特殊群体,科学研究也证实海洛因成瘾者多具有习惯性说谎等反社会人格障碍的个性行为特征。由于长期的海洛因依赖,海洛因成瘾者的大脑皮层普遍受到损伤,致使其包括执行功能在内的高级认知功能发生异常,同时表现出大脑神经递质系统功能紊乱,导致海洛因成瘾者出现注意力缺失、延迟奖赏折扣率等高冲动性特点。为了降低海洛因成瘾对社会的危害,改善成瘾者的身心健康状况,目前我国采取了社区维持治疗的举措,对海洛因成瘾者实施以药物戒治为主的生理一心理—社会综合干预。但是,参加社区药物维持治疗后海洛因戒治者的高级认知功能是否得到改善,目前尚有争论。鉴于此,对海洛因戒治者说谎行为中所涉及的执行功能及冲动性的变化进行研究具有较为重要的现实意义和理论意义。隐藏信息测试(Concealed Information Test, CIT)是经典测谎范式,可以从无关信息中区分出与犯罪事件相关的关键信息,从而判断一个人是否“有罪”。本研究在一个更为宏观的思路指导下,使用基于事件相关电位(event-related potential, ER(P)技术的隐藏信息测试范式,首次考察海洛因戒治者说谎所涉及的执行功能及冲动性的异常变化,并提供神经生物学角度的解释。整个研究由四个系列研究共7个实验组成。
     研究一考察在隐藏信息测试中海洛因戒治者与正常对照人群之间的大脑皮层时程动态变化差异,包括隐藏信息测试及实施反测试措施的隐藏信息测试2个子实验。实验1A采用隐藏信息测试范式,比较海洛因戒治者与正常健康人群在隐藏信息条件下的行为及脑电差异。行为操作结果发现,与对照组相比较,海洛因戒治组被试的反应时更短、正确率更低,具有显著差异;ERP数据显示,在隐藏信息加工过程中,海洛因戒治者被试三类条件诱发的ERP波幅均显著小于对照组;对照组被试探测刺激的ERP成分P3a(300-450ms)与P3b(450-650ms)的平均波幅均大于无关刺激,而海洛因戒治组被试探测刺激的P3a波幅显著小于无关刺激,P3b则与无关刺激无显著差异;海洛因戒治组的隐藏信息测试检测率(38.9%)低于对照组(76.7%)。实验1A的结果说明,海洛因戒治者因长期的慢性海洛因暴露而出现注意朝向缺陷及决策功能障碍,因此他们更容易逃脱隐藏信息测试的侦测。依据已有研究结果,在隐藏信息测试中对无关刺激实施反测试措施会使侦测率降低,在实验1A的基础上,实验1B比较海洛因戒治者与正常健康人群在实施反测试措施的条件下的行为与脑电差异。结果发现,与未选图片相比较,两组均的已选图片均反应时更长,正确率更低。ERP结果显示,在隐藏信息的反测试加工过程中,海洛因戒治者被试三类条件诱发的ERP波幅均显著小于对照组;与对照组相反,海洛因戒治组被试探测刺激的P3a与P3b平均波幅均小于无关刺激;海洛因戒治组的隐藏信息测试检测率(20%)低于对照组(33.3%)。结果表明实施反测试手段后,海洛因戒治者的注意朝向加工能力减弱,与说谎相关的刺激差异性降低,隐藏信息测试的敏感性降低,海洛因戒治者因而更容易逃避隐藏信息测试的检测。
     研究二考察不同冲动性水平海洛因戒治者在隐藏信息测试中的大脑皮层动态时程变化差异,包括隐藏信息测试及反测试2个子实验。实验2A比较不同冲动性水平的两组海洛因戒治者被试在隐藏信息条件下的行为与脑电差异,结果发现,冲动性水平高的海洛因戒治组被试探测刺激的正确率更低,探测刺激的P3a与P3b波幅与无关刺激之间的差异均显著性降低,出现更为严重的注意朝向缺失和决策功能异常;高冲动组的隐藏信息测试检测率(30.8%)低于低冲动组组(50%)。实验2B比较冲动性水平不同的两组海洛因戒治者被试在隐藏信息条件下反测试的行为与脑电差异,结果发现,较冲动性水平低的戒治者而言,高冲动性戒治者探测刺激的反应时更长、正确率更低,探测刺激的P3a与P3b波幅与无关刺激之间差异消失,表明注意朝向和决策功能均受损更为严重;高冲动组的隐藏信息测试检测率(0%)低于低冲动组组(38.5%)。研究二表明,在隐藏信息测试与反测试中冲动性水平的高低对海洛因戒治者说谎相关的认知功能均有一定影响,冲动性水平越高,认知功能受损越严重越容易逃脱隐藏信息测试及反测试的检测。
     研究三通过考察海洛因戒治者执行功能大脑皮层时间进程的异常,以及冲动性水平对执行功能的影响来解释海洛因戒治者隐藏信息测试中诊断率低的认知加工原因,包括2个子实验。实验3A对比海洛因戒治者与正常健康人群执行控制功能,发现在经典色词Stroop任务中,海洛因戒治组的N450与SP效应消失,说明海洛因戒治者存在早期的刺激监控及晚期的冲突解决等加工异常,进一步解释了海洛因戒治者在隐藏信息测试中不易被侦测的原因。为探讨冲动性对执行控制功能的影响,在实验3A的基础上,实验3B对比冲动性水平高低不同的两组海洛因戒治者被试执行功能的脑电差异。发现冲动性水平低的海洛因戒治组被试的N450效应和SP效应均出现衰减,而相比较之下,高冲动性水平海洛因戒治组被试的这两种效应都消失。结果说明冲动性越高,海洛因戒治者的执行控制功能受损程度越严重,这进一步可以解释在研究二隐藏信息测试、反测试过程中,与低冲动海洛因戒治者相比较,高冲动海洛因戒治者的注意朝向缺失和决策功能异常的程度均比更为严重,同时表现出更低水平的测试检测率。
     中枢神经递质的活动是人类行为的神经生物学基础之一,神经递质功能的异常会导致个体冲动性水平的异常。研究四探讨海洛因戒治者与正常健康人群之间神经递质变化的差异,进一步解释海洛因戒治者冲动性水平高于正常人群,为研究一和研究二提供神经生物学证据。实验使用脑涨落图仪,检测了海洛因戒治组和对照组被试的γ-氨基丁酸(GABA)、谷氨酸(Glu)、五羟色胺(5-HT)、乙酰胆碱(ACh)、去甲肾上腺素(NE)、多巴胺(DA)等六种神经递质的实测功率、相对功率以及全脑总功率和脑功能指数。结果发现,与对照组相比较,海洛因戒治组被试的GABA、Glu实测功率及相对功率显著降低,5-HT、ACh、NE、DA则显著升高;全脑总功率显著低于对照组,兴奋抑制指数及相对熵显著高于对照组。结果说明海洛因戒治者兴奋性和抑制性的神经递质系统之间的平衡被打破,导致了冲动性明显增加。
     综上所述,本研究可以得到以下几个结论:(1)在隐藏信息测试和反测试中,海洛因戒治者存在注意朝向缺失和决策功能异常,更易逃脱隐藏信息测试与反测试的检测。(2)不同冲动性水平的海洛因戒治者在隐藏信息测试和反测试中也存在程度不同的注意朝向缺失和决策功能异常,随着冲动性水平升高,这些认知功能受损的程度更为严重,更易成功隐藏相关信息而不被检测出来。(3)在经典色词Stroop任务中,海洛因戒治者表现刺激冲突监控和冲突解决的异常,在高冲动性水平戒治者中这些执行功能受损程度更为严重。因此,冲动性水平的增强应该是海洛因戒治者的执行功能受损的原因之一。(4)来自神经递质系统的证据表明,海洛因戒治者的中枢神经递质系统功能紊乱,兴奋性神经递质和抑制性神经递质之间失去平衡,从而导致戒治者的冲动性水平升高。
     本研究首次揭示了海洛因戒治者在隐藏信息测试及反测试中的大脑皮层时间进程的变化特征,并探讨了冲动性对海洛因戒治者隐藏信息测试中执行功能的影响。此外,通过来自神经递质的证据证实了海洛因戒治者冲动性增强的神经生物学原因。这些结果对进一步探讨海洛因戒治者的高级认知功能具有一定的启示。
Deception is a common social phenomenon, involving advanced cognitive processing functions such as attention, memory, execution control, and so on. Heroin addicts have always been considered a habit of lying even special pathological liar group, scientific researches have also confirmed that the heroin addicts have antisocial personality disorder behavior characteristics, including the habit of lying. Due to long-term heroin dependence, the brain functions of heroin addicts are mostly damaged, leading to their abnormal advanced cognitive functions including executive function. At the same time, heroin addicts show the dysfunctional neurotransmitter system in the brain, leading to their highly impulsive characteristics such as attention deficit, high delayed reward discount rate, and so on. In order to reduce the harm to society, improve physical and mental health for heroin addicts, at present our country take measures that are given priority to with drug treatment of comprehensive physical-psychological-social intervention on heroin addicts. However, at present there have been some debates whether the advanced cognitive processing functions can be improved after those heroin addicts participate in the community maintain treatment treatment. In view of this, there are important practical and theoretical significance to study executive function and the influence of impulsive in deception behaviors among the heroin-dependent treatment patients (HTP). Concealed information test(CIT) is a classic deception-detection paradigm, and it can distinguish the crucial crime-ralated information from the irrelevant ones, thereby to judge whether one is "guilty". Under the guidance of a more macroscopical thinking frame, this study inspected executive dysfunction during their deception process using event related potential technology. The whole research consists of four series studies, a total of seven experiments.
     Study1investigated the differences of dynamic changes of brain time process between the HTPs and the healthy normal people in control group, which consists of two experiments in concealed information test and taking countermeasures to the concealed information test. Experiment1compared behavioral and ERP differences under the condition of concealed information between the HTPs and the healthy normal people in control group. The behavioral results showed that HTP significantly had slower reaction time and lower accurate rate comparing with the control group; ERP data showed that the ERP average amplitudes evoked by the three types of stimuli were significantly lower in HTP group than in the control group; the probe P3a(300-450ms) and P3b(450-650ms) average amplitudes were bigger than these of irrelevant stimuli in the controls, while the probe P3a was smaller than irrelevant and there was no difference for P3b in the HTP group. In addition, the detection rate in HTP group(38.9%) was also significantly lower than in the control group(76.7%). The results in Experiment1A suggested that the long-term chronic heroin exposure can lead HTPs to the deficiency of attention orientation and the disorder of decision-making function, so that they were hard to be detected correctly in the concealed information test. According to the previous research results that performing countermeasures in response to irrelevant stimuli could decrease the detection rate in the concealed information test, on the basis of Experiment1A, Experiment1B compared behavioral and ERP differences between the HTPs and the healthy normal people in control group under the condition of countermeasures in concealed information test. The results found that there were the trends of shorter reaction time and lower accuracy for probe than irrelevant stimuli in the pariticipants of both groups; in the HTP group, the P300average amplitudes evoked by the three types of stimuli reduced significantly, and the probe P3a and P3b average amplitudes were both smaller than these of the irrelevant contrary to the control group. In addition, the detection rate in HTP group(20%) was also lower than in the control group(33.3%). This suggested that the process capability of ateentional orientation is weaken, the difference of the deception-related stimuli is reduced, and HTPs can eacape the concealed information test more easily compared with the controls after performing the countermeasures to the irrelevant stimuli.
     Study2investigated the dynamic time-course change differences of brain cortex from different levels of impulsivity during HTPs were in process of deception. It consisted of two serial experiments. Experiment2A compared the differences of the behavioral and ERP data between two groups of HTPs whose impulsivity were at different levels under the condition of information concealed in concealed information test. The results showed that high impulsive HTPs had more lower accuracy compared with the low impulsivity group, and the Pa3differences between the probe and irrelevant stimuli reduced significantly as well as P3b; the detection rate of high impulsive HTP group(30.8%) was lower than which of the low group(50%). It suggested that there were more serious attention orientation deficit and abnormal decision-making in the high impulsive HTPs than in low impulsive HTPs. Experiment2B compared the differences of the behavioral and ERP data between two groups of HTPs whose impulsivity were at different levels under the condition of countermeasures in concealed information test. The results showed that the slower reaction time and lower accuracy for probe than irrelevant stimuli in the high impulsive HTP group compared the low impulsive group, and the difference of P3a amplitude disappeared between probe and irrelevant stimuli as well as P3b; the detection rate of high impulsive HTP group(0%) was lower than which of the low group(38.5%). It suggested that there are more seriously impaired attention orientation and decision-making function for the high impulsive HTPs than the low impulsive ones. It implies that impulsivity has influence on the cognitive functions in the concealed information test as well as in countermeasure test, and the more higher impulsivity is, the more seriously impaired the cognitive functions are, the more easily the deception can eacape from the concealed information and its countermeasure test.
     Study3included two serial experiments to explain habitual deception and low deception diagnostic rate among HTPs by means of inspecting their abnormal brain time-course processing and the influence of impulsivity on their excutive function. Experiment3A compared the excutive function of the HTP group with the control group, and found that the N450and SP effects were all disappeared for the participants in the HTP group in the classic color-word Stroop task. This suggested that HTPs exsit anomaly during early stimulus monitoring and late conflict sovling, and explained why the HTPs are hard to be detect successfully in the concealed information test. Based on Experiment3A, Experiment3B compared the differences of behavioral and ERP data between two group from different levels of impulsivity, and found that N450and SP effects were all attenuation in the low level group, while all disappeared in the high level group. The results showed that the more higher the impulsivity level was, the more seriously the excutive function was damaged, and explained further why the attention orientation deficiency and decision-making function were more abnormal and detection rate were more lower in the concealed information test and the countermeasure test in the higher impulsivity lvevl HTPs compared with the lower impulsivity level ones in Study2.
     The central neuretransmitter activity is one of the neurobiological bases for human behaviors, and the dynfunctional neurotransmitters can result in the abnormal impulvisity for the individuals. Study4discussed the differences of neurotransmitters between the HTPs and the normal healthy people, explained further why the impulsivity level of HTPs was higher than the nomals, and provided the neurobiological evidence for Study1and2. The experiment employed encephalofluctuograph (EFG) to detect the measure power, the relative power, the whole brain total power and the index of brain function from six kinds of neurotransmitters, such as y-aminobutyric acid (GABA), glutamic acid (Glu), serotonin (5-HT), acetyl choline (ACh), norepinephrine (NE), dopamine (DA) and so on. The results showed that compared the control group, the measure power and relative power of GABA and Glu, and the whole brain total power reduced remarkably in HTP group, but5-HT, Ach, NE, DA, the excitatory-inhibitory index and the relative entropy all rised significantly. The results illustrated that the balance between the excitatory and inhibitory neurotransmitter systems was destroyed, and it caused impulsivity to rise markedly in HTPs.
     In sum, the present research can get following conclusions.(1) In the classical deception detection and countermeasure detection, HTPs exsit abnormal attention orientation and decision-making function, which results in their escape eaily from these tests.(2) To varying degrees, the different levels of impulsive HTPs have abnormal attention orientation and decision-making function, and these cognitive functions are damaged more seriously with the impulsivity level rising in the classical deception detection and countermeasure detection, and deception can escape successfully from these tests.(3) In the classical color-word Stroop task, the HTPs showed abnormal stimulus conflict monitoring and conflict solving, and these excutive functions are impaired more seriously. Therefore, the rising impulsivity is one of reasons why the excutive functions of HTPs are impaired.(4) The neurobiological evidences suggest that the central neurotransmitter system is dysfunction, and the balance between the excitatory and inhibitory neurotransmitter systems is destroyed, resulting in the impulsive level rising markedly in HTPs.
     The present reseatch reveals the features of dynamic change of the brain time-course in the concealed information test and the countermeasure test among the HTPs, and discusses the influence of impulsivity on the excutive function while the HTPs are deceiving. Furthermore, the evidences from nuerontransmitter system comfirm the neurobiological reason of enhanced impulsivity for the HTPs. These results inspire to discuss the advanced cognitive functions further among the HTPs.
引文
Abe, N. (2011). How the Brain Shapes Deception An Integrated Review of the Literature. The Neuroscientist.17(5), 560-574.
    Abe, N., Suzuki, M., Tsukiura, T., Mori. E., Yamaguchi, K.,Itoh, M.,& Fujii, T. (2006). Dissociable roles of prefrontal and anterior cingulate cortices in deception. Cerebral Cortex,16(2),192-199.
    Allan, A., Galindo, R., Chynoweth, J., Engel, S.,& Savage. D. (2001). Conditioned place preference for cocaine is attenuated in mice over-expressing the 5-HT3 receptor. Psychopharmacology,158(1),18-27.
    Allen. J. J.,& Mertens, R. (2009). Limitations to the detection of deception:true and false recollections are poorly distinguished using an event-related potential procedure. Social neuroscience,4(6).473-490.
    Allen, T. J., Moeller, F. G., Rhoades. H. M.,& Cherek, D. R. (1998). Impulsivity and history of drug dependence. Drug and alcohol dependence,50(2),137-145.
    Ambach, W., Stephanie, B., Rudolf, S.,& Dieter, V. (2010). A concealed information test with multimodal measurement. International Journal ofPsychophysiology,75,258-267.
    American Psychiatric Association (Ed.). (2000). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders:DSM-IV-TR(?). American Psychiatric Pub.
    Anand, P., Springer, S. A., Copenhaver, M. M.,& Altice, F. L. (2010). Neurocognitive impairment and HIV risk factors: a reciprocal relationship. AIDS and Behavioral,14(6),1213-1226.
    Aron, A. R. (2007). The neural basis of inhibition in cognitive control. The Neuroscientist,13(3),214-228.
    Aron, A. R., Durston, S., Eagle, D. M., Logan, G. D., Stinear, C. M.,& Stuphorn, V. (2007). Converging evidence for a fronto-basal-ganglia network for inhibitory control of action and cognition. The Journal of Neuroscience,27 (44), 11860-11864.
    Badzakova-Trajkov, G., Barnett, K. J., Waldie, K. E.,& Kirk, I. J. (2009). An ERP investigation of the Stroop task:The role of the cingulate in attentional allocation and conflict resolution. Brain research,1253,139-148.
    Baldacchino, A., Balfour, D. J. K., Passetti, F., Humphris, G.,& Matthews, K. (2012). Neuropsychological consequences of chronic opioid use:A quantitative review and meta-analysis. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews,36(9):2056-2068.
    Balodis, I. M., Potenza, M. N.,& Olmstead, M. C. (2009). Binge drinking in undergraduates:Relationships with gender, drinking behaviors, impulsivity and the perceived effects of alcohol. Behavioural pharmacology,20(5-6),518.
    Banta-Green, C. J., Maynard, C., Koepsell, T. D., Wells, E. A.,& Donovan, D. M. (2009). Retention in methadone maintenance drug treatment for prescription-type opioid primary users compared to heroin users. Addiction, 104(5),775-783.
    Barbelivien, A., Billy, E., Lazarus, C., Kelche, C.,& Majchrzak, M. (2008). Rats with different profiles of impulsive choice behavior exhibit differences in responses to caffeine and< i> d-amphetamine and in medial prefrontal cortex 5-HT utilization. Behavioural brain research,187,273-283.
    Barch, D. M., Braver, T. S., Akbudak, E., Conturo, T., Ollinger, J..& Snyder, A. (2001). Anterior cingulate cortex and response conflict:effects of response modality and processing domain. Cerebral Cortex.11(9),837-848.
    Battisti, R. A., Roodenrys, S., Johnstone, S. J., Pesa, N., Hermens, D. F.,& Solowij, N. (2010). Chronic cannabis users show altered neurophysiological functioning on Stroop task conflict resolution. Psychopharmacology,212(4), 613-624.
    Baumgartner, T., Fischbacher, U., Feierabend, A., Lutz, K.,& Fehr, E. (2009). The neural circuitry of a broken promise. Neuron,64(5),756-770.
    Bechara, A. (2005). Decision making, impulse control and loss of willpower to resist drugs:a neurocognitive perspective. Nature neuroscience,8(11),1458-1463.
    Bechara, A., Damasio, A. R., Damasio, H.,& Anderson, S. W. (1994). Insensitivity to future consequences following damage to human prefrontal cortex. Cognition,50(1),7-15.
    Belin, D., Mar, A. C., Dalley, J. W., Robbins, T. W.,& Everitt, B. J. (2008). High impulsivity predicts the switch to compulsive cocaine-taking. Science,320(5881),1352-1355.
    Ben-Shakhar, G.,& Elaad, E. (2003). The validity of psychophysiological detection of information with the Guilty Knowledge Test:a meta-analytic review. Journal of Applied Psychology,88(1),131.
    Berkman, E. T, Falk, E. B.,& Lieberman, M. D. (2011). In the Trenches of Real-World Self-Control Neural Correlates of Breaking the Link Between Craving and Smoking. Psychological Science,22(4),498-506.
    Berlin, H. A., Rolls, E. T.,& Kischka, U. (2004). Impulsivity, time perception, emotion and reinforcement sensitivity in patients with orbitofrontal cortex lesions. Brain,127(5),1108-1126.
    Bickel, W. K., Jarmolowicz, D. P., Mueller, E. T., Gatchalian, K. M.,& McClure, S. M. (2012). Are executive function and impulsivity antipodes? A conceptual reconstruction with special reference to addiction. Psychopharmacology,221(3),361-387.
    Blum, K., Gardner, E., Oscar-Berman, M.,& Gold, M. (2012). "Liking" and "wanting" linked to Reward Deficiency Syndrome (RDS):Hypothesizing differential responsivity in brain reward circuitry. Current pharmaceutical design,18(1),113-118.
    Boaz, T. L., Perry, N. W., Raney, G, Fiseher, I. S.,& Shuman, D. (1991). Detection of guilty knowledge with event-related potential. Journal of Applied Psychology,76,788-795.
    Bonelli, R. M.,& Cummings, J. L. (2007). Frontal-subcortical circuitry and behavior. Dialogues in clinical neuroscience,9(2),141-151.
    Bornovalova, M. A., Daughters, S. B., Hernandez, G D., Richards, J. B.,& Lejuez, C. W. (2005). Differences in impulsivity and risk-taking propensity between primary users of crack cocaine and primary users of heroin in a residential substance-use program. Experimental and Clinical Psychopharmacology,13(4),311-318.
    Bradley, M. T., Barefoot, C. A.,& Arsenault, A. M. (2011). "Leakage of information to innocent suspects," in Memory Detection:Theory and Application of the Concealed Information Test, eds B. Verschuere, G. Ben-Shakhar,& E. Meijer (Cambridge:Cambridge University Press),187-199.
    Broos, N., Schmaal, L., Wiskerke, J., Kostelijk, L., Lam, T., Stoop, N.,...& Goudriaan, A. E. (2012). The relationship between impulsive choice and impulsive action:a cross-species translational study. PloS one,7(5), e36781.
    Carinci, A. J.,& Mao, J. (2010). Pain and opioid addiction:what is the connection?. Current pain and headache reports,14(1),17-21.
    Carlson, S. R., Johnson, S. C.,& Jacobs, P. C. (2010). Disinhibited characteristics and binge drinking among university student drinkers. Addictive behaviors,35(3),242-251.
    Chen, A.C.H., Porjesz, B., Rangaswamy, M., Kamarajan, C., Tang, Y., Jones, K.A., et al. (2007). Reduced frontal lobe activity in subjects with high impulsivity and alcoholism. Alcoholism. Clinical and Experimental Research,31, 156-165.
    Chen, C. Y, Tien, Y M., Juan, C. H., Tzeng, O. J.,& Hung, D. L. (2005). Neural correlates of impulsive-violent behavior:an event-related potential study. Neuroreport,16(11),1213-1216.
    Chow, S. L. (1988). Significance test or effect size?. Psychological Bulletin,103(1),105-110.
    Christ, S. E., Van Essen, D. C., Watson, J. M, Brubaker, L. E.,& McDermott, K. B. (2009). The contributions of prefrontal cortex and executive control to deception:evidence from activation likelihood estimate meta-analyses. Cerebral Cortex,19(1),1557-1566.
    Clark, L., Robbins, T. W., Ersche, K. D.,& Sahakian, B. J. (2006). Reflection impulsivity in current and former substance users. Biological psychiatry,60(5),515-522.
    Cloninger, C. R. (1994). Temperament and personality. Current opinion in neurobiology,4(2),266-273.
    Cloninger, C. R., Przybeck, T. R.,& Svrakic, D. M. (1991). The tridimensional personality questionnaire:US normative data. Psychological reports,69(3),1047-1057.
    Coffey, S. F., Gudleski, G D., Saladin, M. E.,& Brady, K. T. (2003). Impulsivity and rapid discounting of delayed hypothetical rewards in cocaine-dependent individuals. Experimental and clinical psychopharmacology,11,18-25.
    Collette, F.,& Van der Linden, M. (2002). Brain imaging of the central executive component of working memory. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews,26(2),105-125.
    Congdon, E.,& Canli, T. (2008). A neurogenetic approach to impulsivity. Journal of personality,76(6),1447-1484.
    Connolly, C. G, Foxe, J. J., Nierenberg, J., Shpaner, M.,& Garavan, H. (2012). The neurobiology of cognitive control in successful cocaine abstinence. Drug and alcohol dependence,121(1),45-53.
    Cormier, E. (2008). Attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder:a review and update. Journal of pediatric nursing,23(5), 345-357.
    Coskunpinar, A.,& Cyders, M. A. (2013). Impulsivity and substance-related attentional bias:A meta-analytic review. Drug and alcohol dependence,133(1),1-14.
    Cox, W. M., Hogan, L. M., Kristian, M. R.,& Race, J. H. (2002). Alcohol attentional bias as a predictor of alcohol abusers'treatment outcome. Drug and alcohol dependence,68(3),237-243.
    Cox, W. M., Pothos, E. M.,& Hosier, S. G. (2007). Cognitive-motivational predictors of excessive drinkers'success in changing. Psychopharmacology,192(4),499-510.
    Crews, F. T.,& Boettiger, C. A. (2009). Impulsivity, frontal lobes and risk for addiction. Pharmacology Biochemistry and Behavior,93(3),237-247.
    Curran, H. V., Kleckham, J., Bearn, J., Strang, J.,& Wanigaratne, S. (2001). Effects of methadone on cognition, mood and craving in detoxifying opiate addicts:a dose-response study. Psychopharmacology,154(2),153-160.
    Cyders, M. A.,& Smith, G T. (2007). Mood-based rash action and its components:Positive and negative urgency. Personality and Individual Differences,43(4),839-850.
    Dalley, J. W., Everitt, B. J.,& Robbins, T. W. (2011). Impulsivity, compulsivity, and top-down cognitive control. Neuron,69(4),680-694.
    Dalley, J. W., Fryer, T. D., Brichard, L., Robinson, E. S., Theobald, D. E., Laane, K.,...& Robbins, T. W. (2007). Nucleus accumbens D2/3 receptors predict trait impulsivity and cocaine reinforcement, science,315(5816), 1267-1270.
    Danielmeier, C., Wessel, J. R., Steinhauser, M.,& Ullsperger, M. (2009). Modulation of the error-related negativity by response conflict.Psychophysiology,46(6),1288-1298.
    Dawe, S., Gullo, M. J.,& Loxton, N. J. (2004). Reward drive and rash impulsiveness as dimensions of impulsivity: Implications for substance misuse. Addictive behaviors,29(1),1389-1405.
    De Pascalis, V., Strippoli, E., Riccardi, P.,& Vergari, F., (2004). Personality, event-related potential (ERP) and heart rate (HR) in emotional word processing. Personality and Individual Differences 36,873-891.
    De Rover, M., Lodder, J. C., Schoffelmeer, A. N.,& Brussaard, A. B. (2005). Intermittent morphine treatment induces a long-lasting increase in cholinergic modulation of GABAergic synapses in nucleus accumbens of adult rats. Synapse,55(1),17-25.
    de Wit, H. (2009). Impulsivity as a determinant and consequence of drug use:a review of underlying processes. Addiction biology,14(1),22-31.
    Dom, G., De Wilde, B., Hulstijn, W., Van Den Brink, W.,& Sabbe, B. (2006). Decision-making deficits in alcohol-dependent patients with and without comorbid personality disorder. Alcoholism:Clinical and Experimental Research,30(10),1670-1677.
    Donkers, F. C. L.,& van Boxtel, G. J. M. (2004). The N2 in go/no-go tasks rdects conflict monitoring not response inhibition. Brain and Cognition,56,165-176.
    Donohue, S. E., Liotti, M., Perez III, R.,& Woldorff, M. G. (2012). Is conflict monitoring supramodal? Spatiotemporal dynamics of cognitive control processes in an auditory Stroop task. Cognitive, Affective,& Behavioral Neuroscience,12,1-15.
    Dougherty, D. M., Bjork, J. M., Andrew Harper, R., Marsh, D. M., Gerard Moeller, F., Mathias, C. W.,& Swann, A. C. (2003). Behavioral impulsivity paradigms:a comparison in hospitalized adolescents with disruptive behavior disorders. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry,44(8),1145-1157.
    Eagle, D. M., Tufft, M. R., Goodchild, H. L.,& Robbins, T. W. (2007). Differential effects of modafinil and methylphenidate on stop-signal reaction time task performance in the rat, and interactions with the dopamine receptor antagonist cis-flupenthixol. Psychopharmacology,192(2),193-206.
    Egner, T.,& Hirsch, J. (2005). Cognitive control mechanisms resolve conflict through cortical amplification of task-relevant information. Nature neuroscience,8(12),1784-1790.
    Elaad, E. (1998). The challenge of the concealed knowledge polygraph test.Expert Evidence,6(3),161-187.
    Elaad, E. (2009). Effects of context and state of guilt on the detection of concealed crime information. International Journal ofPsychophysiology,71(3),225-234.
    Ersche, K. D., Clark, L., London, M., Robbins, T. W.,& Sahakian, B. J. (2006). Profile of executive and memory function associated with amphetamine and opiate dependence. Neuropsychopharmacology,31,1036-1047.
    Eysenck, S. B., Pearson, P. R., Easting, G.,& Allsopp, J. F. (1985). Age norms for impulsiveness, venturesomeness and empathy in adults.Personality and individual differences,6(5),613-619.
    Falkenstein, M. (2006). Inhibition, conflict and the Nogo-N2. Clinical Neurophysiology,117(8),1638-1640.
    Fallgatter, A. J.,& Strik, W. K. (1999). The NoGo-anteriorization as a neurophysiological standard-index for cognitive response control.International Journal ofPsychophysiology,32(3),233-238.
    Farwell, L. A. (2002). Method and apparatus for brainfingerprinting measurement, assessment and analysis of brain function. WIPO Patent No.2002100241. Geneva. Switzerland:World Intellectual Property Organization.
    Farwell. L. A. (2008). Brain fingerprinting detects real crimes in the field despite one-hundred-thousand-dollar reward for beating it. Psychophysiology,45(s1).
    Farwell. L. A. (2010). Method for brain fingerprinting, measurement, assessment and analysis of brain function. US Patent,# 7,689,272. Washington, DC:U.S. Patent and Trademark Office.
    Farwell, L.A. (2011). Brain fingerprinting:corrections to Rosenfeld. Scientific Review of Mental Health Practice.8,
    Farwell, L. A. (in press.2012a). Scientific consensus on brain fingerprinting and differing views on the science, technology, and application. Cognitive Neurodynamics.
    Farwell, L. A. (in press,2012b). Bnfingerprinting:a comprehensive tutorial review of detection of concealed information with event-related brain potentials. Cognitive Neurodynamics.
    Farwell, L. A.,& Donchin, E. (1991). The Truth Will Out:Interrogative Polygraphy ("Lie Detection") With Event-Related Brain Potentials. Psychophysiology.28(5),531-547.
    Farwell. L. A., Richardson. D. C.& Richardson, G M. (2013). Brain fingerprinting field studies comparing P300-MERMER and P300 brainwave responses in the detection of concealed information. Cognitive Neurodynamics,7(4),263-299.
    Farwell, L.A.,& Smith, S. S. (2001). Using brain MERMER testing to detect concealed knowledge despite efforts to conceal. Journal of Forensic Sciences,46,135-143.
    Fassbender, C., Hester, R., Murphy, K., Foxe, J. J., Foxe, D. M.,& Garavan, H. (2009). Prefrontal and midline interactions mediating behavioural control. European Journal ofNeuroscience,29(1),181-187.
    Feil, J., Sheppard, D., Fitzgerald, P. B., Yucel, M., Lubman, D. I.,& Bradshaw, J. L. (2010). Addiction, compulsive drug seeking, and the role of frontostriatal mechanisms in regulating inhibitory control. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews,35(2),248-275.
    Field, M.,& Cox, W. M. (2008). Attentional bias in addictive behaviors:a review of its development, causes. and consequences. Drug and alcohol dependence,97(1),1-20.
    First, M. B., Frances, A.,& Pincus, H. A. (2002). DSM-IV-TR handbook of differential diagnosis. American Psychiatric Publishing, Inc..
    Fishbein, D. H., Krupitsky, E., Flannery, B. A., Langevin, D. J., Bobashev, G., Verbitskaya, E.,...& Tsoy, M. (2007). Neurocognitive characterizations of Russian heroin addicts without a significant history of other drug use. Drug and alcohol dependence,90(1).25-38.
    Floresco, S. B., Tse, M. T.,& Ghods-Sharifi, S. (2007). Dopaminergic and glutamatergic regulation of effort-and delay-based decision making.Neuropsychopharmacology,33(8),1966-1979.
    Folstein, J. R.,& van Petten, C. (2008). Influence of cognitive control and mismatch on the N2 component of the ERP: a review. Psychophysiology,45,152-170.
    Forster, S. E., Carter, C. S., Cohen, J. D.,& Cho, R. Y. (2011). Parametric manipulation of the conflict signal and control-state adaptation. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience,23(4),923-935.
    Franken, I. H., Stam, C.J., Ilendriks, V. M.,& van den Brink, W. (2003). Neurophysiological evidence for abnormal cognitive processing of drug cues in heroin dependence. Psychopharmacology,170(2),205-212.
    Freedman, M., Black, S., Ebert, P.,& Binns, M. (1998). Orbitofrontal function, object alternation and perseveration. Cerebral Cortex,8(1),18-27.
    Friedland, G (2010). Infectious disease comorbidities adversely affecting substance users with HIV:hepatitis C and tuberculosis. JAIDS Journal of Acquired Immune Deficiency Syndromes,55, S37-S42.
    Fu, L. P., Bi, G H., Zou, Z. T., Wang, Y., Ye, E. M., Ma, L.,& Yang, Z. (2008). Impaired response inhibition function in abstinent heroin dependents:an fMRI study. Neuroscience letters,438(3),322-326.
    Gajewski, P. D., Stoerig, P.,& Falkenstein, M. (2008). ERP-correlates of response selection in a response cfliet paradigm. Brain Research,1189,127-134.
    Gamer, M.,& Berti, S. (2010). Task relevance and recognition of concealed information have different influences on electrodermal activity and event-related brain potentials. Psychophysiology,47(2),355-364.
    Gamer, M., Klimecki, O., Bauermann, T., Stoeter, P.,& Vossel, G (2012). fMRI-activation patterns in the detection of concealed information rely on memory-related effects. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience,7(5), 506-515.
    Garavan, H.,& Stout, J. C. (2005). Neurocognitive insights into substance abuse. Trends in cognitive sciences,9(4), 195-201.
    Giordano, L. A., Bickel, W. K., Loewenstein, G, Jacobs, E. A., Marsch, L.,& Badger, G. J. (2002). Mild opioid deprivation increases the degree that opioid-dependent outpatients discount delayed heroin and money.Psychopharmacology,163(2),174-182.
    Goldstein, R. Z.,& Volkow, N. D. (2002). Drug addiction and its underlying neurobiological basis:neuroimaging evidence for the involvement of the frontal cortex. The American journal of psychiatry,159(10),1642.
    Gonsalvez, C. J.,& Polich, J. (2002). P300 amplitude is determined by target-to-target interval. Psychophysiology, 39(3):388-396.
    Gossop, M., Trakada, K., Stewart, D.,& Witton, J. (2005). Reductions in criminal convictions after addiction treatment: 5-year follow-up. Drug and Alcohol Dependence,79(3),295-302.
    Graham, D. L., Hoppenot, R., Hendryx, A.,& Self, D. W. (2007). Differential ability of D1 and D2 dopamine receptor agonists to induce and modulate expression and reinstatement of cocaine place preference in rats. Psychopharmacology,191(3),719-730.
    Greene, J. D.,& Paxton, J. M. (2009). Patterns of neural activity associated with honest and dishonest moral decisions. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences,106(30),12506-12511.
    Grier, J. B. (1971). Nonparametric indexes for sensitivity and bias:computing formulas. Psychological bulletin,75(6), 424-429.
    Gronau, N., Ben-Shakhar, G,& Cohen, A. (2005). Behavioral and physiological measures in the detection of concealed information. Journal of Applied Psychology,90(1),147.
    Gruber, S. A., Tzilos, G K., Silveri, M. M., Pollack, M., Renshaw, P. F., Kaufman, M. J.,& Yurgelun-Todd, D. A. (2006). Methadone maintenance improves cognitive performance after two months of treatment. Experimental and Clinical Psychopharmacology,14(2),157-164.
    Hanslmayr, S., Pastotter, B., Bauml, K. H., Gruber, S., Wimber, M.,& Klimesch, W. (2008). The electrophysiological dynamics of interference during the Stroop task. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience,20(2),215-225.
    Hare, R. D. (2003). Manual for the Hare psychopathy checklist. Gozden gecirilmis,2.
    Hermann, D., Frischknecht, U., Heinrich, M., Hoerst, M., Vollmert, C., Vollstadt-Klein, S.,...& Ende, G (2012). MR spectroscopy in opiate maintenance therapy:association of glutamate with the number of previous withdrawals in the anterior cingulate cortex. Addiction biology,17(3).659-667.
    Hester, R., Lubman, D. I.,& Yucel, M. (2010). The role of executive control in human drug addiction. InBehavioral Neuroscience of Drug Addiction (3:301-318). Springer Berlin Heidelberg.
    Hester, R., Nestor, L.,& Garavan. H. (2009). Impaired error awareness and anterior cingulate cortex hypoactivity in chronic cannabis users. Neuropsychopharmacology,34(11),2450-2458.
    Higley, J. D.,& Linnoila, M. (1997). Low central nervous system serotonergic activity is traitlike and correlates with impulsive behavior. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences,836(1),39-56.
    Iacono, W. G,& McGue, M. (2006). Association between P3 event-related brain potential amplitude and adolescent problem behavior. Psychophysiology.43(5),465-469.
    Jentsch, J. D., Taylor, J. R. (1999) Impulsivity resulting from frontostriatal dysfunction in drug abuse:implications for the control of behavior by reward-related stimuli. Psychopharmacology (Berl),146,373-390.
    Jokinen, A., Santtila, P., Ravaja,N.,& Puttonen, S. (2006). Salience of guilty knowledgetest items affects accuracy in realistic mock crimes. International Journal of Psychophysiology,62,175-184.
    Kam, J. W., Dominelli, R.,& Carlson, S. R. (2012). Differential relationships between sub-traits of BIS-11 impulsivity and executive processes:An ERP study. International Journal of Psychophysiology,85(2),174-187.
    Kanske, P.,& Kotz, S. A. (2010). Modulation of early conflict processing:N200 responses to emotional words in a flanker task. Neuropsychologia,48(12),3661-3664.
    Kheramin, S., Body, S., Ho, M. Y., Velazquez-Martinez, D. N., Bradshaw, C. M., Szabadi, E.,...& Anderson,I. M. (2004). Effects of orbital prefrontal cortex dopamine depletion on inter-temporal choice:a quantitative analysis.Psychopharmacology,175,206-214.
    Kirby, K. N.,& Petry, N. M. (2004). Heroin and cocaine abusers have higher discount rates for delayed rewards than alcoholics or non-drug-using controls. Addiction,99(4),461-471.
    Kiyatkin, E. A. (2002). Dopamine in the nucleus accumbens:cellular actions, drug-and behavior-associated fluctuations, and a possible role in an organism's adaptive activity. Behavioural brain research,137(1),27-46.
    Kleiner, M. (Ed.). (2002). Handbook of polygraph testing. San Diego, CA:Academic Press,265-286.
    Koob, G. F..& Volkow, N. D. (2010). Neurocircuitry of addiction. Neuropsychopharmacology,35,217-238.
    Krapohl, D. J. (2011). "Limitations of the concealed information test in criminal cases," in Memory Detection:Theory and Application of the Concealed Information Test, eds B. Verschuere, G. Ben-Shakhar,& E. Meijer (Cambridge: Cambridge University Press),151-170.
    Krapohl, D.& Sturm, S. (2002). Terminology reference for the science of psychophysiological detection of deception. Polygraph quarterly,31(3),154-239.
    Kreek, M. J., Borg, L., Ducat, E.,& Ray, B. (2010). Pharmacotherapy in the treatment of addiction:methadone. Journal of addictive diseases,29(2),200-216.
    Kubo, K.,& Nittono, H. (2008). Detecting the intention to conceal the truth:An event-related potential study. Proceedings,55-58.
    Kutas, M.,& Federmeier, K. D. (2011). Thirty years and counting:Finding meaning in the N400 component of the event-related brain potential (ERP). Annual review of psychology,62,621-647.
    Kutas, M.,& Hillyard. S. A. (1980). Reading senseless sentences:brain potential reflect semantic incongruity. Science, 207:203-205.
    Labkovsky, E.,& Rosenfeld, J. P.(2011). The P300-based, complex trial protocol for concealed information detection resists any number of sequential countermeasures against up to five irrelevant stimuli. Applied Psychophysiology Biofeedback,37,1-10.
    Lamar, M., Cutter, W., Rubia, K., Brammer, M., Daly, E., Craig, M., Cleare, A.J., Murphy, D. (2007).5-HT, prefrontal function and aging:fMRI of inhibition and acute tryptophan depletion. Neurobiology of aging,30(7),1135-1146.
    Langleben, D. D., Schroeder, L., Maldjian, J. A., Gur, R. C., McDonald, s., Ragland, J. D., et al. (2002). Brain activity during simulated deception:an event-related functional magnetic resonance study. Neurolmage,15,727-732.
    Larson, M. J., Farrer, T. J.,& Clayson, P. E. (2011). Cognitive control in mild traumatic brain injury:Conflict monitoring and conflict adaptation. International Journal of Psychophysiology,82(1),69-78.
    Larson, M. J., Kaufman, D. A.,& Perlstein, W. M. (2009). Conflict adaptation and cognitive control adjustments following traumatic brain injury. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society,15(06),927-937.
    Lee, T.,& Pau, C. W. (2002). Impulse control differences between abstinent heroin users and matched controls. Brain Injury,16(10),885-889.
    Lee, T., Zhou, W. H., Luo, X. J., Yuen, K. S., Ruan, X. Z.,& Weng, X. C. (2005). Neural activity associated with cognitive regulation in heroin users:A fMRI study. Neuroscience letters,382(3),211-216.
    Le Foll, B., Gallo, A., Le Strat, Y., Lu, L.,& Gorwood, P. (2009). Genetics of dopamine receptors and drug addiction:a comprehensive review. Behavioural pharmacology,20(1),1-17.
    Lefebvre, C. D. (2006). The use of event-related brain potentials (ERPs) to assess eyewitness idenfication accuracy. Unpublished doctoral thesis. Halifax, NS:Dalhousie University.
    Lefebvre, C. D., Marchand, Y., Smith, S.M.,& Connolly, J.F. (2001). Determining eyewitness idenfication accuracy using event-related brain potentials (ERPs). Psychophysiology,44,894-904.
    Lejuez, C. W., Paulson, A., Daughters, S. B., Bornovalova, M. A.,& Zvolensky, M. J. (2006). The association between heroin use and anxiety sensitivity among inner-city individuals in residential drug use treatment. Behaviour research and therapy,44(5),667-677.
    Li, Q., Yang, W. C., Wang, Y. R., Huang, Y. F., LI, W., Zhu, J.,...& Tian, J. (2013). Abnormal function of the posterior cingulate cortex in heroin addicted users during resting-state and drug-cue stimulation task. Chinese medical journal,126(4),734-739.
    Liotti, M., Woldorff, M.G., Perez III, R.,& Mayberg, H.S. (2000). An ERP study of the temporal course of the Stroop color-word interference effect. Neuropsychologia,38,701-711.
    Liu, Q. S., Pu, L.,& Poo, M. M. (2005). Repeated cocaine exposure in vivo facilitates LTP induction in midbrain dopamine neurons. Nature,437(7061),1027-1031.
    Loeber, S., Kniest, A., Diehl, A., Mann, K.,& Croissant, B. (2008). Neuropsychological functioning of opiate-dependent patients:a nonrandomized comparison of patients preferring either buprenorphine or methadone maintenance treatment. The American journal of drug and alcohol abuse,34(5),584-593.
    Lu, L., Fang, Y,& Wang, X. (2008). Drug abuse in China:past, present and future. Cellular and molecular neurobiology,28(4),479-490.
    Lubman, D. I., Yucel, M.,& Pantelis, C. (2004). Addiction, a condition of compulsive behaviour? Neuroimaging and neuropsychological evidence of inhibitory dysregulation. Addiction,99,1491-1502.
    Luck, S. J. (2005). An introduction to the event-related potential technique (cognitive neuroscience). Cambridge. MA: MIT Press
    Lui. M.& Rosenfeld, J. P. (2008). Detection of deception about multiple, concealed, mock crime items, based on a spatial-temporal analysis of ERP amplitude and scalp distribution. Psychophysiology,45:721-730
    Lykken, D. T. (1959). The GSR in the detection of guilt. Journal of Applied Psychology,43(6),385.
    Lyoo,I. K., Pollack, M. H., Silveri, M. M. Ahn, K. H., Diaz, C. I., Hwang, J.,...& Renshaw, P. F. (2006). Prefrontal and temporal gray matter density decreases in opiate dependence. Psychopharmacology,184(2),139-144.
    MacLaren, V. V. (2001). A quantitative review of the guilty knowledge test. Journal of applied psychology,86(4), 674-683.
    MacLeod, C. M. (1991). Half a century of research on the Stroop effect:an integrative review. Psychological bulletin,109(2).163.-203.
    Ma, N., Liu, Y., Li, N., Wang, C. X., Zhang, H., Jiang. X. F.,...& Zhang, D. R. (2010). Addiction related alteration in resting-state brain connectivity.Neuroimage,49(1),738-744.
    Markela-Lerenc. J., Ille, N., Kaiser, S., Fiedler, P., Mundt, C.& Weisbrod, M. (2004). Prefrontal-cingulate activation during executive control:which comes first?. Cognitive Brain Research,18(3),278-287.
    Matsuda, I., Nittono, H.,& Allen, J. J. (2012). The current and future status of the concealed information test for field use. Frontiers in psychology,3,1-11.
    Matsuda, I., Nittono, H., Hirota, A., Ogawa, T.,& Takasawa, N. (2009). Event-related brain potentials during the standard autonomic-based concealed information test. International Journal of Psychophysiology,74(1),58-68.
    Mattick, R. P., Breen, C., Kimber, J.,& Davoli, M. (2009). Methadone maintenance therapy versus no opioid replacement therapy for opioid dependence. Cochrane Database System Review,3.
    Meegan, D. V. (2008). Neuroimaging techniques for memory detection:scientific, ethical, and legal issues. The American Journal of Bioethics,8,9-20.
    Meijer, E., Smulders, F. T., Merckelbach, H.,& Wolf, A. (2007). The P300 is sensitive to concealed face recognition. International Journal of Psychophysiology,66,231-237.
    Meijer, E., Smulders, F. T..& Wolf, A. (2009). The contribution of mere recognition to the P300 effect in a concealed information test. Applied Psychophysiology and Biofeedback. 34(3):221-226.
    Meixner, J. B.,& Rosenfeld. J. P. (2010). Countermeasure mechanisms in a P300-based concealed information test. Psychophysiology,47,57-65.
    Meixner, J. B.,& Rosenfeld, J. P. (2011). A mock terrorism application of the P300-based concealed information test. Psychophysiology,48,149-154.
    Mertens. R.,& Allen, J. J. (2008). The role of psychophysiology in forensic assessments:Deception detection, ERPs, and virtual reality mock crime scenarios. Psychophysiology,45(2),286-298.
    Mintzer, M. Z., Copersino, M. L.,& Stitzer, M. L. (2005). Opioid abuse and cognitive performance. Drug and alcohol dependence,78(2),225-230.
    Miyake, A., Friedman, N. P., Emerson, M. J., Witzki, A. H., Howerter, A.,& Wager, T. D. (2000). The unity and diversity of executive functions and their contributions to complex "frontal lobe" tasks:A latent variable analysis.Cognitive psychology,41(1),49-100.
    Moeller, F. G., Barratt, E. S., Dougherty, D. M., Schmitz, J. M.,& Swann, A. C. (2001). Psychiatric aspects of impulsivity. American journal of psychiatry,158(11),1783-1793.
    Moeller, F. G., Hasan, K. M., Steinberg, J. L., Kramer, L. A., Dougherty. D. M., Santos, R. M.,...& Narayana, P. A. (2004). Reduced anterior corpus callosum white matter integrity is related to increased impulsivity and reduced discriminability in cocaine-dependent subjects:diffusion tensor imaging. Neuropsychopharmacology,30(3),
    Nieuwenhuis, S., Yeung, N., van den Wildenberg, W.,& Ridderinkhof, K. R. (2003). Electrophysiological correlates of anterior cingulated function in a go/no-go task:effects of response coflict and trial type frequency. Cognitive, Affective,& Behavioral Neuroscienee,3,17-26.
    Nunez, J. M., Casey, B. J., Egner, T., Hare, T.,& Hirsch, J. (2005). Intentional false responding shares neural substrates with response conflict and cognitive control. Neuroimage,25(1),267-277.
    Okvist, A., Fagergren. P., Whittard, J., Garcia-Osta, A., Drakenberg, K., Horvath, M. C.,...& Hurd, Y. L. (2011). Dysregulated postsynaptic density and endocytic zone in the amygdala of human heroin and cocaine abusers.Biological psychiatry,69(3),245-252.
    Oviedo-Joekes. E., Brissette. S., Marsh. D. C., Lauzon, P., Guh, D., Anis, A.,& Schechter, M. T. (2009). Diacetylmorphine versus methadone for the treatment of opioid addiction. New England Journal of Medicine,361(8),777-786.
    Palleja, J. M., Martin, E. G.,& Alonso, M. C. H. (2004). The nonverbal approach to the detection of deception: judgemental accuracy. Psychology in Spain. (8),48-59.
    Papageorgiou, C. C., Liappas, I. A., Ventouras, E. M., Nikolaou, C. C., Kitsonas, E. N., Uzunoglu, N. K.,& Rabavilas, A. D. (2004). Long-term abstinence syndrome in heroin addicts:indices of P300 alterations associated with a short memory task. Progress in Neuro-Psychopharmacology and Biological Psychiatry,28(1),1109-1115.
    Passetti, F., Clark. L., Mehta, M. A., Joyce, E.,& King, M. (2008). Neuropsychological predictors of clinical outcome in opiate addiction. Drug and Alcohol Dependence,94(1).82-91.
    Pattij, T.,& De Vries, T. J. (2013). The role of impulsivity in relapse vulnerability. Current opinion in neurobiology,23(4).700-705.
    Pattij, T., Schetters, D., Janssen, M. C., Wiskerke, J.,& Schoffelmeer, A. N. (2009). Acute effects of morphine on distinct forms of impulsive behavior in rats. Psychopharmacology,205(3),489-502.
    Pattij, T.,& Vanderschuren, L. J. (2008). The neuropharmacology of impulsive behaviour. Trends in Pharmacological Sciences,29(4),192-199.
    Patton, J. H.,& Stanford, M. S. (1995). Factor structure of the Barratt impulsiveness scale. Journal of clinical psychology,51(6),768-774.
    Payte, J. T.,& Zweben, J. E. (1998). Opioid maintenance therapies.Principles of addiction medicine.2nd ed. Chevy-Chase (MD):American society of addiction medicine,557-570.
    Perry, J. L.,& Carroll, M. E. (2008). The role of impulsive behavior in drug abuse. Psychopharmacology.200(\),1-26.
    Polich, J. (2007). Updating P300:an integrative theory of P3a and P3b. Clinical neurophysiology:official journal of the International Federation of Clinical. Neurophysiology,118(10):2128-2148.
    Potcnza, M. N.,& Taylor, J. R. (2009). Found in translation:Understanding impulsivity and related constructs through integrative preclinical and clinical research. Biological psychiatry,66(8).714.
    Priori, A., Mameli, F., Cogiamanian, F., Marceglia. S., Tiriticco. M., Mrakic-Sposta, S.,...& Sartori, G. (2008). Lie-specific involvement of dorsolateral prefrontal cortex in deception. Cerebral Cortex,18(2),451-455.
    Prosser, J.. Cohen, L. J., Steinfeld, M.. Eisenberg, D., London, E. D.,& Galynker, I. I. (2006). Neuropsychological functioning in opiate-dependent subjects receiving and following methadone maintenance treatment. Drug and alcohol dependence,84(3),240-247.
    Rapeli. P.. Fabritius, C., Alho, H., Salaspuro, M., Wahlbeck, K.,& Kalska. H. (2007). Methadone vs. buprenorphine/naloxone during early opioid substitution treatment:a naturalistic comparison of cognitive performance relative to healthy controls. BMC Pharmacology and Toxicology,7(1),5-15.
    Rapeli, P., Fabritius. C., Kalska. H.,& Alho, H. (2011). Cognitive functioning in opioid-dependent patients treated with buprenorphine, methadone, and other psychoactive medications:stability and correlates. BMC Pharmacology and Toxicology,11.13-27.
    Reimer, J., Verthein, U., Karow, A.. Schafer, I., Naber, D.,& Haasen, C. (2011). Physical and mental health in severe opioid-dependent patients within a randomized controlled maintenance treatment trial. Addiction.106(9), 1647-1655.
    Reynolds, B. (2006). A review of delay-discounting research with humans:relations to drug use and gambling. Behavioural pharmacology.17(8),651-667.
    Robinson, T. E.,& Kolb, B. (2004). Structural plasticity associated with exposure to drugs of abuse. Neuropharmacology,47,33-46.
    Rogers, R. D., Everitt, B. J., Baldacchino, A., Blackshaw, A. J., Swainson, R., Wynne, K., et al. (1999). Dissociable deficits in the decision-making cognition of chronic amphetamine abusers, opiate abusers, patients with focal damage to prefrontal cortex, and tryptophan-depleted normal volunteers:Evidence for monoaminergic mechanisms. Neuropsychopharmacology,20,322-339.
    Rogers, R. D., Ramnani, N., Mackay, C., Wilson, J. L., Jezzard, P., Carter, C. S.,& Smith, S. M. (2004). Distinct portions of anterior cingulate cortex and medial prefrontal cortex are activated by reward processing in separable phases of decision-making cognition. Biological psychiatry,55(6),594-602.
    Rosenfeld, J. P. (2005). Brain fingerprinting:A critical analysis. Scientific Review of Mental Health Practice,4(1),
    Rosenfeld, J. P. (2011). P300 in detecting concealed information. In B. Verschuere, G. Ben Shakhar,& E. Meijer (Eds.), Memory detection:Theory and application of the Concealed Information Test (pp.63-89). Cambridge:Cambridge University Press.
    Rosenfeld, J. P., Biroschak, J. R.,& Furedy, J. J. (2006). P300-based detection of concealed autobiographical versus incidentally acquired information in target and non-target paradigms. International Journal of Psychophysiology,60(3),251-259.
    Rosenfeld, J. P., Cantwell, B., Nasman, V. T.. Wojdac, V., Ivanov, S.,& Mazzeri, L. (1988). A modified, event-related potential-based guilty knowledge test. International Journal ofNeuroscience,42(1-2).157-161.
    Rosenfeld, J.,& Labkovsky, E. (2010). New P300-based protocol to detect concealed information:resistance to mental countermeasures against only half the irrelevant stimuli and a possible ERP indicator of countermeasures. Psychophysiology,47,1002-1010.
    Rosenfeld, J. P., Labkovsky, E., Winograd, M., Lui, M. A., Vandenboom, C.,& Chedid, E. (2008). The Complex Trial Protocol (CTP):A new, countermeasure-resistant, accurate, P300-based method for detection of concealed information. Psychophysiology,45(6),906-919.
    Rosenfeld, J. P., Shue, E.,& Singer, E. (2007). Single versus multiple probe blocks of P300-based concealed information tests for self-referring versus incidentally obtained information. Biological psychology,74(3), 396-404.
    Rosenfeld, J. P., Soskins, M., Bosh, G.,& Ryan, A. (2004). Simple, effective countermeasures to P300-based tests of detection of concealed information. Psychophysiology,41(2),205-219.
    Rubia, K., Lee, F., Cleare, A.J., Tunstall, N., Fu, C.H., Brammer, M.,& McGuire, P. (2005). Tryptophan depletion reduces right inferior prefrontal activation during response inhibition in fast, event-related fMRI. Psychopharmacology,179,791-803.
    Russo, P.M., Pascalis, V., Varriale, V.,& Barratt, E.S., (2008). Impulsivity, intelligence, and P300 wave:an empirical study. International Journal of Psychophysiology 69,112-118.
    Schacter, L. D.,& Slotnick, S. D. (2004). The cognitive neuroscience of memory distortion. Neuron.44,149-160.
    Scheres, A., Lee, A.,& Sumiya, M. (2008). Temporal reward discounting and ADHD:task and symptom specific effects. Journal of Neural Transmission,115,221-226.
    Schmaal, L. (2013). Boosting impulse control in addiction:Pharmacological neuroimaging studies. BOXPress.
    Schmajuk, M., Liotti, M., Busse, L.,& Woldorff, M. G (2006). Electrophysiological activity underlying inhibitory control processes in normal adults. Neuropsychologia,44(3),384-395.
    Schmidt, A., Walter, M., Gerber, H., Schmid, O., Smieskova, R., Bendfeldt, K.,...& Borgwardt, S. (2013). Inferior frontal cortex modulation with an acute dose of heroin during cognitive control. Neuropsychopharmacology, 38,2231-2239.
    Schoenbaum, G, Roesch, M. R.,& Stalnaker, T. A. (2006). Orbitofrontal cortex, decision-making and drug addiction. Trends in neurosciences,29(2),116-124.
    Sequeira, A., Mamdani, F., Ernst, C., Vawter, M. P., Bunney, W. E., Lebel, V,...& Turecki, G (2009). Global brain gene expression analysis links glutamatergic and GABAergic alterations to suicide and major depression.PLoS One,4(8):65-85.
    Seymour, T. L., Seifert, C. M., Shafto, M. G,& Mosmann, A. L. (2000). Using response time measures to assess" guilty knowledge". Journal of Applied Psychology,85(1),30-37.
    Shuster, S. (2011). The curse of the crocodile:Russia's deadly designer drug. Time (World) June20.
    Silton, R. L., Heller, W., Towers, D. N., Engels, A. S., Spielberg, J. M., Edgar, J. C.,...& Miller, G A. (2010). The time course of activity in dorsolateral prefrontal cortex and anterior cingulate cortex during top-down attentional control. Neuroimage,50(3),1292-1302.
    Sim, M. E., Lyoo, I. K., Streeter, C. C., Covell, J., Sarid-Segal, O., Ciraulo, D. A.,...& Renshaw, P. F. (2007). Cerebellar gray matter volume correlates with duration of cocaine use in cocaine-dependent subjects. Neuropsychopharmacology,32(10),2229-2237.
    Singh, S. M., Basu, D., Kohli, A.,& Prabhakar, S. (2009). Auditory P300 Event-Related Potentials and Neurocognitive Functions in Opioid Dependent Men and Their Brothers. The American Journal on Addictions,18(3),198-205.
    Sip, K. E., Roepstorff, A., McGregor, W.,& Frith, C. D. (2008). Detecting deception:the scope and limits. Trends in cognitive sciences,12(2),48-53.
    Smith, J. L., Johnstone, S. J.,& Barry, R. J. (2004). Inhibitory processing during the Go/NoGo task:an ERP analysis of children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Clinical Neurophysiology,115(6),1320-1331.
    Soyka, M., Limmer, C., Lehnert, R., Koller, G., Martin. G., Kufner. H.,...& Haberthur, A. (2011). A comparison of cognitive function in patients under maintenance treatment with heroin, methadone, or buprenorphine and healthy controls:an open pilot study. The American Journal of Drug and Alcohol Abuse,37(6),497-508.
    Soyka. M., Zingg, C. Koller, G.,& Hennig-Fast, K. (2010). Cognitive function in short-and long-term substitution treatment:Are there differences?. World Journal of Biological Psychiatry,11 (2_2),400-408.
    Soyka, M., Zingg, C., Koller, G.,& Kuefner, H. (2008). Retention rate and substance use in methadone and buprenorphine maintenance therapy and predictors of outcome:results from a randomized study. The International Journal of Neuropsychopharmacology,11(5).641.
    Sequeira, A., Mamdani, F., Ernst, C., Vawter, M. P., Bunney, W. E., Lebel, V.,...& Turecki, G. (2009). Global brain gene expression analysis links glutamatergic and GABAergic alterations to suicide and major depression.PLoS One,4(8), e6585.
    Spence, S. A., Hunter, M. D., Farrow, T. F., Green, R. D., Leung, D. H., Hughes, C. J.,& Ganesan, V. (2004). A cognitive neurobiological account of deception:evidence from functional neuroimaging. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci,359(1451),1755-1762.
    Stanford, M. S., Mathias, C. W., Dougherty, D. M., Lake, S. L., Anderson, N. E.,& Patton, J. H. (2009). Fifty years of the Barratt Impulsiveness Scale:An update and review. Personality and Individual Differences,47(5),385-395.
    Stockman, J. K.,& Strathdee, S. A. (2010). HIV among people who use drugs:a global perspective of populations at risk. Journal of acquired immune deficiency syndromes (1999),55(Suppl 1), S17-S22.
    Strathdee, S. A.,& Stockman, J. K. (2010). Epidemiology of HIV among injecting and non-injecting drug users:current trends and implications for interventions. Current HIV/AIDS Reports,7(2),99-106.
    Streeter, C. C., Terhune, D. B., Whitfield, T. H., Gruber, S., Sarid-Segal, O., Silveri, M. M,...& Yurgelun-Todd, D. A. (2008). Performance on the Stroop predicts treatment compliance in cocaine-dependent individuals. Neuropsychopharmacology,33(4),827-836.
    Stroop, J. R. (1935). Studies of interference in serial verbal reactions. Journal of experimental psychology,18(6),643.
    Stuss, D. T., Gallup, G G.,& Alexander, M. P. (2001). The frontal lobes are necessary fortheory of mind'. Brain,124(2), 279-286.
    Sun, D., Chan, C. C. H.,& Lee, T. M C. (2012). Identification and classification of facial familiarity in directed lying: an ERP study. PLoS ONE,7:e31250. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0031250.
    Swann, A. C., Lijffijt, M., Lane, S. D., Steinberg, J. L.,& Moeller, F. G (2009). Increased traitike impulsivity and course of illness in bipolar disorder. Bipolar disorders,11(3),280-288.
    Szucs, D.,& Soltesz, F. (2010). Stimulus and response conflict in the color-word Stroop task:a combined electro-myography and event-related potential study. Brain research,1325,63-76.
    Szucs, D., Soltesz, F.,& White, S. (2009). Motor conflict in Stroop tasks:Direct evidence from single-trial electro-myography and electro-encephalography. Neurolmage,47,1960-1973.
    van den Bergh. F. S.. Bloemarts. E., Groenink, L., Olivier. B.,& Costing. R. S. (2006). Delay aversion:Effects of 7-OH-DPAT,5-HT< sub> 1 A/1B-rcceptor stimulation and d-cycloserine. Pharmacology Biochemistry and Behavior,85(4),736-743.
    Vansickel, A. R., Fillmorex, M. T., Hays, L. R..& Rush, C. R. (2008). Effects of potential agonist-replacement therapies for stimulant dependence on inhibitory control in cocaine abusers. The American journal of drug and alcohol abuse,34,293-305.
    Vassileva, J., Gonzalez, R., Bechara, A.,& Martin, E. M. (2007). Are all drug addicts impulsive? Effects of antisociality and extent of multidrug use on cognitive and motor impulsivity. Addictive behaviors.32(12),3071-3076.
    Vendemia, J. M. (2003). Neural Mechanisms of Deception and Response Congruity to General Knowledge Information and Autobiographical Information in Visual Two-Stimulus Paradigms with Motor Response. Department of Defense Polygraph Institute (DoDPI99-P-0010).
    Vendemia, J. M., Buzan, R. F.,& Green, E. P. (2005). Practice effects, workload, and reaction time in deception. The American Journal of Psychology,118(3):413-429.
    Vendemia, J. M., Buzan, R. F., Green, E. P.,& Schillaci, M. J. (2006). Effects of Preparedness to Deceive on ERP Waveforms in a Two-Stimulus Paradigm. Journal of Neurotherapy,9(3),45-70.
    Vendemia, J. M., Buzan, R. F..& Simon-Dack S. L. (2005)R. eaction time of motor responses in two-stimulus paradigms involving deception and congruity with varying levels of difficulty. Behavioural Neurology,16(1):
    Verdejo, A., Toribio, I., Orozco, C., Puente, K. L.,& Perez-Garcia, M. (2005). Neuropsychological functioning in methadone maintenance patients versus abstinent heroin abusers. Drug and Alcohol Dependence,78(3),283-288.
    Verdejo-Garcia, A.,& Bechara, A. (2009). A somatic marker theory of addiction. Neuropharmacology,56,48-62.
    Verdejo-Garcia, A., Lawrence, A. J.,& Clark, L. (2008). Impulsivity as a vulnerability marker for substance-use disorders:review of findings from high-risk research, problem gamblers and genetic association studies. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews,32(4),777-810.
    Verdejo-Garcia, A., Lopez-Torrecillas. F., Gimenez, C. O.,& Perez-Garcia, M. (2004). Clinical implications and methodological challenges in the study of the neuropsychological correlates of cannabis, stimulant, and opioid abuse. Neuropsychology review,14,1-41.
    Verdejo-Garcia, A.,& Perez-Garcia, M. (2008). Substance abusers'self-awareness of the neurobehavioral consequences of addiction. Psychiatry research,158(2),172-180.
    Verdejo-Garcia, A., Perales, J. C.,& Perez-Garcia, M. (2007). Cognitive impulsivity in cocaine and heroin polysubstance abusers. Addictive behaviors,32(5),950-966.
    Verdejo, A., Toribio, I., Orozco. C., Puente, K. L.,& Perez-Garcia, M. (2005). Neuropsychological functioning in methadone maintenance patients versus abstinent heroin abusers. Drug and Alcohol Dependence,78(3),283-288.
    Verschuere, B., Ben-Shakhar, G.,& Meijer, E. (Eds.). (2011). Memory detection:theory and application of the concealed information test. Cambridge University Press.63-89.
    Verschuere, B., Crombez, G., De Clercq, A.,& Koster, E. H. W. (2004). Autonomic and behavioral responding to concealed information:differentiating orienting and defensive responses. Psychophysiology,41,461-466.
    Verschuere, B., Crombez, G., Koster, E. H.,& Uzieblo, K. (2006). Psychopathy and physiological detection of concealed information:A review. Psychologica belgica,46(1-2),99-116.
    Volkow, N. D.. Fowler, J. S.,& Wang, G. J. (2003). The addicted human brain:insights from imaging studies. Journal of Clinical Investigation,111(10),1444-1451.
    Volkow. N. D.. Fowler, J. S.,& Wang, G. J. (2004). The addicted human brain viewed in the light of imaging studies: brain circuits and treatment strategies. Neuropharmacology,47,3-13.
    Voon, V., Reynolds, B., Brezing, C., Gallea, C., Skaljic, M., Ekanayake, V.,...& Hallett, M. (2010). Impulsive choice and response in dopamine agonist-related impulse control behaviors. Psychopharmacology,207(4),645-659.
    Walczyk, J. J., Roper, K.S., Seemann, E.,& Humphrey, A.M. (2003). Cognitive mechanisms underlying lying to questions:response time as a cue to deception. Applied Cognition Psychology,17,755-774.
    Wang, Y., Zhu, J., Li, Q., Li, W., Wu, N., Zheng, Y.,...& Wang, W. (2013). Altered Fronto-Striatal and Fronto-Cerebellar Circuits in Heroin-Dependent Individuals:A Resting-State fMRI Study. PloS one,8(3), e58098.
    Wasserman, S.,& Bockenholt, U. (1989). Bootstrapping:Applications to psychophysiology. Psychophysiology,26(2), 208-221.
    Waters, A. J., Shiffman, S., Bradley, B. P.,& Mogg, K. (2003). Attentional shifts to smoking cues in smokers. Addiction,98(10),1409-1417.
    Wei, D., Qiu, J., Tu, S., Tian, F., Su, Y,& Luo, Y. (2010). Earthquake experience interference effects in a modified Stroop task:an ERP study. Neuroscience letters,474(3),121-125.
    West, R. (2004). The effects of aging on controlled attention and conflict processing in the Stroop task. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience,16(1),103-113.
    West, R.,& Alain, C. (2000). Effects of task context and fluctuations of attention on neural activity supporting performance of the Stroop task. Brain research,873(1),102-111.
    West, R., Jakubek, K., Wymbs, N., Perry, M.,& Moore, K. (2005). Neural correlates of conflict processing. Experimental Brain Research,167(1),38-48.
    West, R.,& Moore, K. (2005). Adjustments of cognitive control in younger and older adults. Cortex,41(4),570-581.
    White, J. M.,& Lopatko, O. V. (2007). Opioid maintenance:a comparative review of pharmacological strategies. Expert Opinion on Pharmacotherapy,8(1):1-11.
    Whiteside, S. P.,& Lynam, D. R. (2001). The five factor model and impulsivity:Using a structural model of personality to understand impulsivity. Personality and individual differences,30(4),669-689.
    Winograd, M. R.,& Rosenfeld, J. P. (2011). Mock crime application of the Complex Trial Protocol (CTP) P300-based concealed information test. Psychophysiology,48,155-161.
    Winstanley, C. A. (2007). The orbitofrontal cortex, impulsivity, and addiction. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences,1121(1),639-655.
    Winstanley, C. A., Dalley, J. W., Theobald, D. E.,& Robbins, T. W. (2003). Global 5-HT depletion attenuates the ability of amphetamine to decrease impulsive choice on a delay-discounting task in rats. Psychopharmacology,170(3), 320-331.
    Winstanley, C. A., Eagle, D. M.,& Robbins, T. W. (2006). Behavioral models of impulsivity in relation to ADHD: translation between clinical and preclinical studies. Clinical psychology review,26(4),379-395.
    Winstanley, C. A., Theobald, D. E., Cardinal, R. N.,& Robbins, T. W. (2004). Contrasting roles of basolateral amygdala and orbitofrontal cortex in impulsive choice. The Journal of neuroscience,24,4718-4722.
    Xi, Z. X., Yang, Z., Li, S. J., Li, X., Dillon, C., Peng, X. Q.,...& Gardner, E. L. (2007). Levo-tetrahydropalmatine inhibits cocaine's rewarding effects:experiments with self-administration and brain-stimulation reward in rats.Neuropharmacology,53(6),771-782.
    Xiao, Z., Lee, T., Zhang, J. X., Wu, Q., Wu, R., Weng, X.,& Hu, X. (2006). Thirsty heroin addicts show different fMRI activations when exposed to water-related and drug-related cues. Drug and alcohol dependence,83(2),157-162.
    Yang, B., Yang, S., Zhao, L., Yin, L., Liu, X.,& An, S. (2009). Event-related potentials in a Go/Nogo task of abnormal response inhibition in heroin addicts. Science in China Series C:Life Sciences,52(8),780-788.
    Yang, Y, Raine, A., Narr, K. L., Lenez, T., LaCASSE, L. O. R. I., Colletti, P.,& Toga, A. W. (2007). Localisation of increased prefrontal white matter in pathological liars. The British Journal of Psychiatry,190(2),174-175.
    YUcel, M., Lubman, D. I., Harrison, B. J., Fornito, A., Allen, N. B., Wellard, R. M.,...& Pantelis, C. (2007b). A combined spectroscopic and functional MRI investigation of the dorsal anterior cingulate region in opiate addiction.Molecular psychiatry,12(1),691-702.
    Yiicel, M., Lubman, D.I., Solowij, N.,& Brewer, W.J. (2007a). Understanding drug addiction:a neuropsychological perspective. Australian and New Zealand Journal of Psychiatry, 41(12):957-968.
    Zeeb, F. D., Floresco, S. B.,& Winstanley, C. A. (2010). Contributions of the orbitofrontal cortex to impulsive choice: interactions with basal levels of impulsivity, dopamine signalling, and reward-related cues.Psychopharmacology,211(1),87-98.
    Zeeb, F. D.,& Winstanley, C. A. (2011). Lesions of the basolateral amygdala and orbitofrontal cortex differentially affect acquisition and performance of a rodent gambling task. The Journal of Neuroscience,31(6),2197-2204.
    Zeng, H., Lee, T. M., Waters, J. H., So, K. F., Sham, P. C., Schottenfeld, R. S.,...& Chawarski, M. C. (2013). Impulsivity, cognitive function, and their relationship in heroin-dependent individuals. Journal of clinical and experimental neuropsychology,35(9),897-905.
    Zuckerman, M., Kuhlman, D. M., Joireman, J., Teta, P.,& Kraft, M. (1993). A comparison of three structural models for personality:the Big Three, the Big Five, and the Alternative Five. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,65(4),757-768崔茜.2013.欺骗的神经机制和测谎新指标初探.西南大学博士学位论文.
    刁秋霞,李积胜.(2008).阿片依赖相关神经递质研究进展.武警医学院学报,17(2),152-154.
    董珊珊,陈飞燕, & 何宏建.(2013).脑成像技术的测的应用及其心理生理学基础.生物物理学报,29(2),94-104.
    何新,周宏兵,李观海.(1994).γ-氨基丁酸与癫痫及其检测方法.西北药学杂志,9:141-143.
    胡传鹏,邓晓红,周治金,邓小刚.(2012).神经法学:年轻的认知神经科学与古老的法学联姻.群学通报,56:3041-3053.
    黄明延,周传岱,白延强,刘月红.(1991).神经衰弱的大脑涨落图分型.中级医刊,26(9),17-19.
    龚栩,谢熹瑶,徐蕊, & 罗跃嘉.(2010).抑郁-焦虑-压力量表简体中文版(DASS-21)在中国大学生中的测试报告.中国临床心理学杂志,18(4),443-446.
    李梦姣,陈杰,李新影.(2012).非药物成瘾的遗传学和神经生物学机制研究述评.心理科学进属20(10),1623-1632
    李秋风,胡建.(2013).5-羟色胺系统与酒依赖关系的研究进展.中华行为医学与脑科学杂志,22,568-570.
    李生斌,朱永生.(2012).冲动与多巴胺系统在毒品依赖中的作用机制.西安交通大学学报(医学版),33(1),1-4.
    罗非,罗劲,吴一兵,丁之光,李佳音,王锦琰, & 郭建友.(2009).脑功能可塑性与灾后心理功能康复.心理科学进展,17(3),594-601.
    蒋晓江,许志强,刘娟.(2005).内因性失眠症患者中枢抑制源GABA震荡的脑电涨落变化.中国临床神经科学,13:236-238,
    社区药物维持治疗国家级工作组秘书处.社区药物维持治疗工作进展报告[R].2012年全国滥用阿片类物质成瘾者社区药物维持治疗工作总结暨现场经验交流会,兰州:2012-12-27-28.
    沈渔邮.(2005).精神病学(第四版).北京:人民卫生出版社,367-373.
    苏亚玲,陈汉华,洪汉林,王风梅,王娜.(2004).海洛因依赖患者明尼苏达多相人格调查表测试结果的聚类分析.中国药物依赖性杂志,13(2),130-132.
    王君,陈天勇.(2012).抑制控制与高级认知功能的关系.心理科学进展,20(11),1768-1778.
    王绪轶,郝伟,李武.(2006).海洛因依赖者选择性注意功能的对照研究.中国行为医学科学,15(4):326-327.
    吴红海,侯艳宁.(2005).大鼠吗啡精神依赖时脑区氨基酸类神经递质的变化.解放军药学学报,21:248-251.
    薛朝霞,李新旺.(2012).中枢多巴胺系统对冲动决策的影响研究进展.中华行为医学与脑科学杂志,20(11),1054-1056.
    殷彬,臧德馨.(2004).海洛因对依赖者去甲肾上腺素和肾上腺素的影响.中国民康医学杂志,16:138-139-190.
    张锋,周艳艳,李鹏,沈模卫.(2008).海洛因戒除者的为冲动性:基于DDT和IGT任务反应模式的探讨.心理学报,40(6),642-653.
    张柳,李新旺,张文婷,杜瑞.(2012).冲动性对吗啡诱导的条件性位置偏爱及行为敏感化的影响.心理学报,44(007),936-943.
    张康莉,张晓明,姚金慧.(2010).自杀倾向者脑内神经递质水平及其与脑功能状态的相关性.中华行为医学与脑科学杂志,19:224-225.
    张雨新,王燕,钱铭怡.(1990).Beck抑郁量表的信度和效度.中国心理卫生杂志,4(4),164-168.
    周亮,肖水源,何晓燕,厉洁,刘慧铭.(2006).BIS-11中文版的信度与效度检验.中国临床心理学杂志,14(4),343-344.
    周文华,杨国栋.(2006).海洛因依赖:一种慢性脑病的临床治疗.中国药物滥用防治杂志,12(5),251-254.
    朱海燕,沈模卫,殷素梅.(2005).不同康复时相戒除者对海洛因相关线索的注意偏向.应用心理学14,297-301.
    朱千,孟景,位东涛,陈红.(2014).海洛因戒治者执行功能异常的电生理证据.心理科学37(2):473-477.
    朱千,明杰,张光群.(2009).心理行为干预对美沙酮门诊病人生活质量的影响研究.现代预防医学,36:3294-3296.
    朱镛连.(2011).神经递质与疾病和药物康复.中国康复理论与实践,17(002),198-200.

© 2004-2018 中国地质图书馆版权所有 京ICP备05064691号 京公网安备11010802017129号

地址:北京市海淀区学院路29号 邮编:100083

电话:办公室:(+86 10)66554848;文献借阅、咨询服务、科技查新:66554700