心理疲劳下运动员反应效果监控的ERP特征
详细信息    本馆镜像全文|  推荐本文 |  |   获取CNKI官网全文
摘要
心理疲劳是心理机能不能维持原有心理活动水平(即下降)的现象;它不利于运动员训练和比赛。而为实现训练和比赛目标,运动员必须做好效果监控,这是指发现和纠正应该做出的反应与实际反应之间的差异(即错误);对自身反应错误的监控叫反应效果监控,它受负性情绪影响较大。心理疲劳(短期和长期)下运动员反应效果监控能力是否下降(ERP核心指标错误相关负波ERN幅值是否变小)有待验证。
     为确定诱发短期心理疲劳和ERN的Flanker任务,选用体育专业大学生以主观感受、行为数据和HRV频域指标为因变量,短期心理疲劳为自变量进行预备性研究。实验1中10人进行6个15分钟分段任务,发现首个15分钟可作为无疲劳期,任务诱发出ERN所需错误数,分段任务不易诱发疲劳。实验2中24人进行60分钟连续任务,发现46-60分钟可作为疲劳期;随后,均分成音乐放松组和对照组进行15分钟任务,发现音乐有助于疲劳恢复。
     研究一以行为数据和ERN为因变量,心理疲劳为自变量探讨了体育专业大学生的心理疲劳与反应效果监控。实验3中15人进行60分钟连续及15分钟音乐调控下的任务,发现无短期疲劳时段正确反应时显著低于(p=.029)、正确率(p=.046)显著高于疲劳时段。ERN幅值上,无短期疲劳显著大于音乐调控时段,后者显著大于疲劳时段(p=.000);早Pe幅值上,无疲劳与调控时段相当,大于疲劳时段。实验4中24人(均分成有、无长期疲劳组)进行15分钟任务,发现无长期疲劳组行为绩效优于长期疲劳组。ERP幅值上,无长期疲劳组ERN(临界显著)和早Pe均大于长期疲劳组。研究一发现ERN最大波幅点是FCz,早Pe较大波幅点是FCz和Cz。
     研究二以行为数据和ERN为因变量,长期心理疲劳为自变量探讨了专业(射击)运动员的长期心理疲劳与反应效果监控。研究二中20人(均分成有、无长期疲劳组)进行15分钟任务,发现正确率、ERN和早Pe有预期方向变化,但两组差异不显著;长期疲劳组出现PES现象。ERN最大波幅点是FCz,早Pe较大波幅点是FCz和Cz。
     本文结论如下:短期和长期心理疲劳均减弱体育专业大学生的反应效果监控能力,放松音乐对短期心理疲劳的调控有一定帮助。长期心理疲劳下专业运动员的反应效果监控能力略有下降,长期心理疲劳对错误后调整有不利影响。
Psychological fatigue, including mental fatigue and burnout, is a phenomenon that psychological function cannot remain the former level of mental activity (In other words, psychological function decreases). Psychological fatigue has some negative effects on athletes'training and competition. However, in order to achieve the goals of training and competition, athletes must do a good job in performance monitoring. Performance monitoring means to detect and correct differences between the right response and the actual response, namely, is to detect and correct errors. Response monitoring reflects error processing from autologous response. Negative emotions affect response monitoring greatly. Under mental fatigue or burnout circumstances, whether athletes'response monitoring ability decreases is up for debate (In ERP studies, the amplitude of error related negativity decreases). Three studies (five experiments) were carried out to explore cause-effect relationships of psychological fatigue and response monitoring.
     The aim of preliminary study was to make certain the Flanker task satisfied to give rise to mental fatigue and ERN. The participants of this study were university students majoring in physical education. This study regarded subjective feelings (difficulty, effort level, degree of fatigue), operational performances (correct rate, reaction time), heart rate variability (frequency domain) as dependent variables, and regarded mental fatigue as independent variable. In experiment1, ten participants operated six separate tasks(Every task was15minutes). The results showed that the first15-minute could be as the non fatigue phase. The Flanker task could evoke a certain amount of errors met the demand of ERN. The six separate tasks could not evoke mental fatigue easily. In experiment2, twenty four participants operated continuously60minute task. The results showed that the fourth15-minute (from46to60minute) could be as the fatigue phase. Then, they were separated averagely into two groups. One group was given relaxed music while the other group was control group. The participants recovered from mental fatigue in the music intervention phase.
     The aim of study1was to investigate the relationship between psychological fatigue and response monitoring. The participants of this study were university students majoring in physical education. This study regarded operational performances (correct rate, reaction time) and ERN as dependent variables, and regarded psychological fatigue as independent variable. In experiment3, fifteen participants operated continuously60minute task, and then operated15minute task accompanied by relaxed music. From the result of operational performances, the fatigue phase compared with the non fatigue phase showed significant differences, including reaction time(p=.029) and correct rate(p=.046). From the result of ERN, the amplitude of the non fatigue phase was bigger than that of the music intervention phase, the latter was bigger than that of the fatigue phase (p=.000). From the result of early Pe, the amplitude of the non fatigue phase equalled basically to that of the music intervention phase, and they were bigger than that of the fatigue phase. In experiment4, twenty four participants were separated averagely into two groups. One group was burnout group while the other group was no burnout group. They operated15minute task. From the result of operational performances, no burnout group was better than burnout group. From the result of ERN, no burnout group's amplitude was bigger than that of burnout group, but the difference was marginally significant(p=.052). From the result of early Pe, no burnout group's amplitude was bigger slightly than that of burnout group. The most obvious electrode was FCz in ERN. The more obvious electrodes were FCz and Cz in early Pe.
     The aim of study2was to investigate the relationship between burnout and response monitoring. The participants of this study were professional athletes (shooters). This study regarded operational performances (correct rate, reaction time) and ERN as dependent variables, and regarded burnout as independent variable. Twenty participants were separated averagely into two groups. One group was burnout group while the other group was no burnout group. They operated15minute task. From the result of operational performances, no burnout group's correct rate was better than that of burnout group. From the result of ERN and early Pe, no burnout group's amplitudes were bigger than those of burnout group, but the differences were not significant. The PES (post-error slowing) phenomenon happened in burnout group. The most obvious electrode was FCz in ERN. The more obvious electrodes were FCz and Cz in early Pe. The main conclusions of this article are as follows:For university students majoring in physical education, Psychological fatigue(mental fatigue and burnout) weakens response monitoring ability. Relaxed music is helpful to mental fatigue recovery. Under burnout circumstances, professional athletes' response monitoring ability declines. Burnout affects post-error adjustment for professional athletes.
引文
毕晓婷.(2009).运动性心力疲劳概念的再解构.硕士学位论文.北京:北京体育大学.
    曹雪亮.(2003).脑力疲劳的注意特征及主观评定方法的实验研究.博士学位论文.西安:第四军医大学.
    方俐洛.(1988).心理负荷概念之概述.心力学动态,6(01),30-34.
    李永鑫,李艺敏(2006).工作倦怠评价标准的初步探讨.心理科学,29(1),148-150.
    李增勇,焦昆,陈铭,王成焘.(2003).汽车驾驶员驾驶过程中的心率变异性功率谱分析.中国生物医学工程学报,22(6),574-576.
    连榕,杨丽娴,吴兰花.(2005).大学生的专业承诺、学习倦怠的关系与量表编制.心理学报,37(5),632-636.
    林岭.(2006).运动性心理疲劳的概念模型、多维检测方法、影响因素及干预措施的实证研究.博士学位论文.北京:北京体育大学.
    刘春雷,张庆林.(2009).错误加工的神经机制.心理科学进展,17(2),341-348.
    刘玉君.(2009).错误后冲突和执行控制对错误后反应的影响.博士学位论文.杭州:浙江大学.
    刘玉丽,张智君.(2008).错误相关负波(ERN)及其理论解释.应用心理学,14(2),180-186.
    马群.(2009).不同程度运动性心理疲劳心率变异性与脑电图特征.硕士学位论文.北京:北京体育大学.
    廖建桥.(1995).脑力负荷及其测量.系统工程学报, 11(03),119-123.
    全国心率变异性分析多中心研究协作组.(2000).心率变异性正常值及其重复性的多中心研究.中华心律失常学杂志,4(03),165-170.
    孙锦绣.(2010).运动性心理疲劳的HRV和ERP特征.博士学位论文.北京:北京体育大学.
    孙锦绣,张力为.(2012).运动员心理疲劳对情绪图片性质判断影响的电生理学特征.体育科学,32(5),58-63,78.
    王瑞元,孙学川,熊开宇,等.(2002).运动生理学北京:人民体育出版社.
    王松涛.(2006).有氧运动对心血管自主神经乎衡状态的影响.博士学位论文.北京:北京体育大学.
    王正伦,杨磊,丁嘉顺.(2005).心率变异性在脑力负荷评价中的应用.中华劳动卫生职业病杂志,23(3),182-184.
    魏景汉,罗跃嘉.(2010).事件相关电位原理与技术.北京:科学出版社.
    晏双平,张进辅,熊春金,甘霖.(2011).心理疲劳发生的认知神经机制.西南师范大学学报(自然科学版),36(03),64-69.
    杨博.(2009).脑力疲劳状态下人脑自动信息加工和冲突信息加工能力变化的ERP研究. 硕士学位论文.西安:第四军医大学.
    张力为.(1992).运动中的心理负荷及其测定.四川体育科学,10(3),32-37.
    张力为, 林岭,赵福兰.(2006,9).运动性心理疲劳的性质测量及控制.首届中国体育博士高层论坛论文集.武汉:武汉体育学院,24-35.
    张力为.(2007,10).心理疲劳与生理疲劳的区分及其意义.第八届全国体育科学大会论文摘要汇编(一).北京:北京体育大学,213-214.
    张力为.(2010).心理学研究的七个方向:以运动性心理疲劳为例.体育科学,30(10),3-12.
    张连成.(2008).技能类项目运动员心理疲劳的评价.硕士学位论文.北京:北京体育大学.
    张连成,张力为.(2010).技能类项目运动员心理疲劳的评价标准.中国体育科技,46(4),105-111.
    Ahsberg, E., Gamberale, R, & Gustafsson, K. (2000). Perceived fatigue after mental work:an experimental evaluation of a fatigue inventory. Ergonomics,43(2),252-268.
    Armbrecht, A. S., Wohrmann, A., Gibbons, H., & Stahl, J. (2010). Temporal dynamics of conflict monitoring and the effects of one or two conflict sources on error-(related) negativity. BiolPsychol,85(1),112-123.
    Barnes, T. C, & Brieger, H. (1946). Electroencephalographic studies of mental fatigue. J Psychol, 22,181-192.
    Bates, A. T., Kiehl, K. A., Laurens, K. R., & Liddle, P. F. (2002). Error-related negativity and correct response negativity in schizophrenia. Clin Neurophysiol,113(9),1454-1463.
    Bechtereva, N. P., & Gretchin, V. B. (1968). Physiological foundations of mental activity. Int Rev Neurobiol,11,329-352.
    Bechtereva, N. P., & Hughes, H. K. (1971). Implanted electrodes:experimentation on humans? Am J Psychiatry,127(10),1422-1423.
    Bechtereva, N. P., Shemyakina, N. V., Starchenko, M. G, Danko, S. G, & Medvedev, S. V. (2005). Error detection mechanisms of the brain:background and prospects. Int J Psychophysiol, 58(2-3),227-234.
    Bechtereva, N. P., & Zontov, V. V. (1962). The relationship between certain forms of potentials and the variations in brain excitability (based on EEG, recorded during photic stimuli triggered by rhythmic brain potentials). Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol,14, 320-330.
    Bills, A. G. (1931). Blocking:A new principle of mental fatigue. American Journal of Psychology, 43,230-245.
    Boksem, M. A., Meijman, T. F., & Lorist, M. M. (2005). Effects of mental fatigue on attention:an ERP study. Cognitive Brain Research,25(1),107-116.
    Boksem, M. A., Meijman, T. F., & Lorist, M. M. (2006). Mental fatigue, motivation and action monitoring. Biol Psychol,72(2),123-132.
    Boksem, M. A., & Tops, M. (2008). Mental fatigue:costs and benefits. Brain research reviews, 59(1),125-139.
    Boksem, M. A., Tops, M., Wester, A. E., Meijman, T. F., & Lorist, M. M. (2006). Error-related ERP components and individual differences in punishment and reward sensitivity. Brain Res,1101(1),92-101.
    Bonnefond, A., Doignon-Camus, N., Hoeft, A., & Dufour, A. (2011). Impact of motivation on cognitive control in the context of vigilance lowering:an ERP study. Brain Cogn,77(3), 464-471.
    Botvinick, M. M., Braver, T. S., Barch, D. M., Carter, C. S., & Cohen, J. D. (2001). Conflict monitoring and cognitive control. Psychol Rev,108(3),624-652.
    Brattico, E., Jacobsen, T., De Baene, W., Glerean, E., & Tervaniemi, M. (2010). Cognitive vs. affective listening modes and judgments of music:an ERP study. Biol Psychol,85(3), 393-409.
    Bush, Q, Luu, P., & Posner, M.I. (2000). Cognitive and emotional influences in anterior cingulate cortex. Trends Cogn Sci,4(6),215-222.
    Caccese, T. M., & Mayerberg, C. K. (1984). Gender differences in perceived burnout of college coaches. Journal of Sport & Exercise Psychology,6(3),279-288.
    Caldwell, G. N., & Riby, L. M. (2007). The effects of music exposure and own genre preference on conscious and unconscious cognitive processes:a pilot ERP study. Conscious Cogn, 16(4),992-996.
    Carrasco, M., Harbin, S. M., Nienhuis, J. K., Fitzgerald, K. D., Gehring, W. J., & Hanna, G L. (2013). Increased error-related brain activity in youth with obsessive compulsive disorder and unaffected siblings. Depression and anxiety,30(1),39-46.
    Cassidy, G G, & Macdonald, R. A. (2010). The effects of music on time perception and performance of a driving game. Scand J Psychol,51(6),455-464.
    Chalder, T., Berelowitz, G, Pawlikowska, T., Watts, L., Wessely, S., Wright, D., & Wallace, E. P. (1993). Development of a fatigue scale. JPsychosom Res,37(2),147-153.
    Chen, J., Yuan, J., Huang, H., Chen, C, & Li, H. (2008). Music-induced mood modulates the strength of emotional negativity bias:an ERP study. Neurosci Lett,445(2),135-139.
    Chiu, P., & Deldin, P. (2007). Neural evidence for enhanced error detection in major depressive disorder. American Journal of Psychiatry,164(4),608-616.
    Coakley, J. A. (1992). Burnout among adolescent athletes:A personal failure or social problem. Sociology of Sport Journal,9(3),271-285.
    Craig, A., Tran, Y., Wijesuriya, N., & Boord, P. (2006). A controlled investigation into the psychological determinants of fatigue. Biological Psychology,72(1),78-87.
    Cresswell, S. L. (2009). Possible early signs of athlete burnout:a prospective study. J Sci Med Sport,12(3),393-398.
    Cresswell, S. L., & Eklund, R. C. (2004). The athlete burnout syndrome:possible early signs. J Sci Med Sport,7(4),481-487.
    Cresswell, S. L., & Eklund, R. C. (2006). The convergent and discriminant validity of burnout measures in sport:a multi-trait/multi-method analysis. J Sports Sci,24(2),209-220.
    Csatho, A., van der Linden, D., Hernadi, I., Buzas, P., & Kalmar, G (2012). Effects of mental fatigue on the capacity limits of visual attention. Journal of Cognitive Psychology,24(5), 511-524.
    Danielmeier, C, Wessel, J. R., Steinhauser, M., & Ullsperger, M. (2009). Modulation of the error-related negativity by response conflict. Psychophysiology,46(6),1288-1298.
    Easdon, C, lzenberg, A., Armilio, M. L., Yu, H., & Alain, C. (2005). Alcohol consumption impairs stimulus-and error-related processing during a Go/No-Go Task. Brain Res Cogn Brain Res,25(3),873-883.
    Egelund, N. (1982). Spectral analysis of heart rate variability as an indicator of driver fatigue. Ergonomics,25(7),663-672.
    Ellis, T. S. (1878). The Position of Rest in Fatigue and in Pain. Br MedJ,l(890),84-85.
    Endrass, T., Reuter, B., & Kathmann, N. (2007). ERP correlates of conscious error recognition: aware and unaware errors in an antisaccade task. Eur JNeurosci,26(6),1714-1720.
    Eriksen, B. A., & Eriksen, C. W. (1974). Effects of noise letters upon the identification of a target letter in a nonsearch task. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics,16(1),143-149.
    Falkenstein, M., Hohnsbein, J., Hoormann, J., & Blanke, L. (1990). Effects of errors in choice reaction tasks on the ERP under focused and divided attention. Psychophysiological brain research,1,192-195.
    Falkenstein, M., Hoormann, J., Christ, S., & Hohnsbein, J. (2000). ERP components on reaction errors and their functional significance:a tutorial. Biological psychology,51(2-3), 87-107.
    Folstein, J. R., & Van Petten, C. (2008). Influence of cognitive control and mismatch on the N2 component of the ERP:a review. Psychophysiology,45(1),152-170.
    Fowler, B. (1994). P300 as a measure of workload during a simulated aircraft landing task. Hum Factors,36(4),670-683.
    Gehring, W. J., Goss, B., Coles, M. G, Meyer, D. E., & Donchin, E. (1993). A Neural System for Error Detection and Compensation. Psychological Science,4(6),385-390.
    Goodger, K., Gorely, T., Lavallee, D., & Harwood, C. (2007). Burnout in sport:A systematic review. Sport Psychologist,21(2),127-151.
    Hajcak, G, & Foti, D. (2008). Errors are aversive:defensive motivation and the error-related negativity. PsycholSci,19(2),103-108.
    Hajcak, G, McDonald, N., & Simons, R. F. (2003). Anxiety and error-related brain activity. Biol Psychol,64(1-2),77-90.
    Hajcak, G, McDonald, N., & Simons, R. F. (2004). Error-related psychophysiology and negative affect. Brain Cogn,56(2),189-197.
    Hajcak, G, Moser, J. S., Yeung, N., & Simons, R. F. (2005). On the ERN and the significance of errors. Psychophysiology,42(2),151-160.
    Hanna, G L., Carrasco, M., Harbin, S. M., Nienhuis, J. K., LaRosa, C. E., Chen, P.,... Gehring, W. J. (2012). Error-related negativity and tic history in pediatric obsessive-compulsive disorder. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry,51(9),902-910.
    Helton, W. S., & Warm, J. S. (2008). Signal salience and the mindlessness theory of vigilance. Acta Psychol (Amst),129(1),18-25.
    Henderson, H., Schwartz, C, Mundy, P., Burnette, C, Sutton, S., Zahka, N., & Pradella, A. (2006). Response monitoring, the error-related negativity, and differences in social behavior in autism. Brain Cogn,61(1),96-109.
    Herrmann, M. J., Rommler, J., Ehlis, A. C, Heidrich, A., & Fallgatter, A. J. (2004). Source localization (LORETA) of the error-related-negativity (ERN/Ne) and positivity (Pe). Brain Res Cogn Brain Res,20(2),294-299.
    Hewig, J., Trippe, R., Hecht, H., Coles, M. G, Holroyd, C. B., & Miltner, W. H. (2007). Decision-making in Blackjack:an electrophysiological analysis. Cereb Cortex,17(4), 865-877.
    Hill, A. P., Hall, H. K., & Appleton, P. R. (2010). Perfectionism and athlete burnout in junior elite athletes:the mediating role of coping tendencies. Anxiety Stress Coping,23(4),415-430.
    Hillman, C. H., Buck, S. M., Themanson, J. R., Pontifex, M. B., & Castelli, D. M. (2009). Aerobic fitness and cognitive development:Event-related brain potential and task performance indices of executive control in preadolescent children. Dev Psychol,45(1),114-129.
    Hillman, C. H., Erickson, K. I., & Kramer, A. F. (2008). Be smart, exercise your heart:exercise effects on brain and cognition. Nat Rev Neurosci,9(1),58-65.
    Hirsh, J. B., & Inzlicht, M. (2010). Error-related negativity predicts academic performance. Psychophysiology,47(1),192-196.
    Hochman, E. Y., Eviatar, Z., Breznitz, Z., Shaul, S., & Nevat, M. (2009). Different laterality patterns of the error-related negativity in corrected and uncorrected errors. Laterality, 14(6),618-634.
    Hodge, K., Lonsdale, C, & Ng, J. Y. (2008). Burnout in elite rugby:relationships with basic psychological needs fulfilment. J Sports Sci,26(8),835-844.
    Hoffmann, S., & Falkenstein, M. (2011). Aging and error processing:age related increase in the variability of the error-negativity is not accompanied by increase in response variability. PLoS One,6(2), e17482.
    Hoffmann, S., Wascher, E., & Falkenstein, M. (2012). Personality and error monitoring:an update. Front Hum Neurosci,6,171.
    Holroyd, C. B., & Coles, M. G (2002). The neural basis of human error processing:reinforcement learning, dopamine, and the error-related negativity. Psychol Rev,109(4),679-709.
    Holroyd, C. B., & Coles, M. G. (2008). Dorsal anterior cingulate cortex integrates reinforcement history to guide voluntary behavior. Cortex,44(5),548-559.
    Holroyd, C. B., Yeung, N., Coles, M. G, & Cohen, J. D. (2005). A mechanism for error detection in speeded response time tasks. JExp Psychol Gen,134(2),163-191.
    Hsieh, S., Li, T. H., & Tsai, L. L. (2010). Impact of monetary incentives on cognitive performance and error monitoring following sleep deprivation. Sleep,33(4),499-507.
    Kamada, T., Miyake, S., Kumashiro, M.. Monou, H., & Inoue, k. (1992). Power spectral analysis of heart rate variability in Type As and Type Bs during mental workload. Psychosomatic Medicine,54(4),462-470.
    Kato, Y., Endo, H., & Kizuka, T. (2009). Mental fatigue and impaired response processes: event-related brain potentials in a Go/NoGo task. International journal of psychophysiology,72(2),204-211.
    Kato, Y, Endo, H., Kobayakawa, T., Kato, K., & Kitazaki, S. (2012). Effects of intermittent odours on cognitive-motor performance and brain functioning during mental fatigue. Ergonomics,55(1),1-11.
    Kim, E. Y., Iwaki, N., Imashioya, H., Uno, H., & Fujita, T. (2007). Error-related negativity in a visual go/no-go task:children vs. adults. Dev Neuropsychol,31(2),181-191.
    Kim, E. Y., Iwaki, N., Uno, H., & Fujita, T. (2005). Error-related negativity in children:effect of an observer. Dev Neuropsychol,28(3),871-883.
    Koelsch, S., Gunter, T. C, Wittfoth, M., & Sammler, D. (2005). Interaction between syntax processing in language and in music:an ERP Study.J Cogn Neurosci,17(10),1565-1577.
    Kraepelin, E. (1902). Die Arbeitscurve [The work curve]. Philosophische Studien,19,459-507.
    Kramer, A. F., Hahn, S., Cohen, N. J., Banich, M. T., McAuley, E., Harrison, C. R.,... Colcombe, A. (1999). Ageing, fitness and neurocognitive function. Nature,400(6143),418-419.
    Kramer, A. F., Trejo, L. J., & Humphrey, D. (1995). Assessment of mental workload with task-irrelevant auditory probes. BiolPsychol,40(1-2),83-100.
    Kurimori, S., Oka, T, & Kakizaki, T. (1994). Evaluation of mental stress and fatigue induced by performance of a mental task.2. An experiment in 11-h self-paced calculating task. Sangyo lgaku,36(1),9-15.
    Laming, D. (1979). Autocorrelation of choice-reaction times. Acta Psychol (Amst),43(5), 381-412.
    Langner, R., Steinborn, M. B., Chatterjee, A., Sturm, W., & Willmes, K. (2010). Mental fatigue and temporal preparation in simple reaction-time performance. Acta Psychol (Amst), 133(1),64-72.
    Larson, M. J., Baldwin, S. A., Good, D. A., & Fair, J. E. (2010). Temporal stability of the error-related negativity (ERN) and post-error positivity (Pe):the role of number of trials. Psychophysiology,47(6),1167-1171.
    Larson, M. J., Perlstein, W. M., Stigge-Kaufman, D., Kelly, K. G, & Dotson, V. M. (2006). Affective context-induced modulation of the error-related negativity. Neuroreport,17(3), 329-333.
    Lee, K. A., Hicks, G, & Nino-Murcia, G (1991). Validity and reliability of a scale to assess fatigue. Psychiatry Res,36(3),291-298.
    Leuthold, H., & Sommer, W. (1999). ERP correlates of error processing in spatial S-R compatibility tasks. Clin Neurophysiol,110(2),342-357.
    Li, Z., Wang, C, Mak, A. F., & Chow, D. H. (2005). Effects of acupuncture on heart rate variability in normal subjects under fatigue and non-fatigue state. Eur J Appl Physiol, 94(5-6),633-640.
    Liu, B., Huang, Y., Wang, Z., & Wu, G. (2012). The influence of background music on recognition processes of Chinese characters:an ERP study. Neurosci Lett,518(2),80-85.
    Lonsdale, C, & Hodge, K. (2011). Temporal ordering of motivational quality and athlete burnout in elite sport. MedSci Sports Exerc,43(5),913-921.
    Lonsdale, C, Hodge, K., & Rose, E. (2009). Athlete burnout in elite sport:a self-determination perspective. J Sports Sci,27(8),785-795.
    Lorist, M. M. (2008). Impact of top-down control during mental fatigue. Brain Res,1232, 113-123.
    Lorist, M. M., Boksem, M. A., & Ridderinkhof, K. R. (2005). Impaired cognitive control and reduced cingulate activity during mental fatigue. Brain Res Cogn Brain Res,24(2), 199-205.
    Lorist, M. M., Klein, M., Nieuwenhuis, S., Jong, R., Mulder, G, & Meijman, T. F. (2000). Mental fatigue and task control:Planning and preparation. Psychophysiology,37(5),614-625.
    Lorist, M. M., & Tops, M. (2003). Caffeine, fatigue, and cognition. Brain Cogn,53(1),82-94.
    Luu, P., Flaisch, T., & Tucker, D. M. (2000). Medial frontal cortex in action monitoring. J Neurosci,20(1),464-469.
    Maier, M., di Pellegrino, G, & Steinhauser, M. (2012). Enhanced error-related negativity on flanker errors:error expectancy or error significance? Psychophysiology,49(7),899-908.
    Maier, M., Steinhauser, M., & Hubner, R. (2008). Is the error-related negativity amplitude related to error detectability? Evidence from effects of different error types. J Cogn Neurosci, 20(12),2263-2273.
    Masaki, H., Falkenstein, M., Sturmer, B., Pinkpank, T., & Sommer, W. (2007). Does the error negativity reflect response conflict strength? Evidence from a Simon task. Psychophysiology,44(4),579-585.
    Masaki, H., Murphy, T. I., Desjardins, J. A., & Segalowitz, S. J. (2012). The error-related negativity associated with different strength of stimulus-response interference. Clin Neurophysiol,123(4),689-699.
    Maslach, C, Schaufeli, W. B., & Leiter, M. P. (2001). Job burnout. Annu Rev Psychol,52, 397-422.
    Mathews, C. A., Perez, V. B., Delucchi, K. L., & Mathalon, D. H. (2012). Error-related negativity in individuals with obsessive-compulsive symptoms:toward an understanding of hoarding behaviors. Biol Psychol,89(2),487-494.
    Meyer, A., Weinberg, A., Klein, D. N., & Hajcak, G (2012). The development of the error-related negativity (ERN) and its relationship with anxiety:evidence from 8 to 13 year-olds. Dev Cogn Neurosci,2(1),152-161.
    Miller, A. E., Watson, J. M., & Strayer, D. L. (2012). Individual differences in working memory capacity predict action monitoring and the error-related negativity. J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn,38(3),757-763.
    Miltner, W. H., Lemke, U., Weiss, T., Holroyd, C, Scheffers, M. K., & Coles, M. G. (2003). Implementation of error-processing in the human anterior cingulate cortex:a source analysis of the magnetic equivalent of the error-related negativity. Biol Psychol,64(1-2), 157-166.
    Mizuno, K., Tanaka, M., Yamaguti, K., Kajimoto, O., Kuratsune, H., & Watanabe, Y. (2011). Mental fatigue caused by prolonged cognitive load associated with sympathetic hyperactivity. Behav Brain Funct,7,17.
    Muller, M., Hofel, L., Brattico, E., & Jacobsen, T. (2010). Aesthetic judgments of music in experts and laypersons-an ERP study. Int J Psychophysiol,76(1),40-51.
    Murata, A., Uetake, A., & Takasawa, Y. (2005). Evaluation of mentalfatigue using feature parameter extracted from event-related potential. International Journal of Industrial Ergonomics,35(8),761-770.
    Nieuwenhuis, S., Ridderinkhof, K. R., Blom, J., Band, G P., & Kok, A. (2001). Error-related brain potentials are differentially related to awareness of response errors:evidence from an antisaccade task. Psychophysiology,38(5),752-760.
    Olvet, D. M., & Hajcak, G. (2009). The effect of trial-to-trial feedback on the error-related negativity and its relationship with anxiety. Cogn Affect Behav Neurosci,9(4),427-433.
    Olvet, D. M., & Hajcak, G. (2012). The error-related negativity relates to sadness following mood induction among individuals with high neuroticism. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci,7(3), 289-295.
    Olvet, D. M., Hatchwell, E., & Hajcak, G (2010). Lack of association between the 5-HTTLPR and the error-related negativity (ERN). Biol Psychol,85(3),504-508.
    Painter, J. G, & Koelsch, S. (2011). Can out-of-context musical sounds convey meaning? An ERP study on the processing of meaning in music. Psychophysiology,48(5),645-655.
    Patel, M., Lai, S. K. L., Kavanagh, D., & Rossiter, P. (2011). Applying neural network analysis on heartratevariability data to assess driver fatigue. Expert Systems with Applications,38(6), 7235-7242.
    Potts, G. F. (2011). Impact of reward and punishment motivation on behavior monitoring as indexed by the error-related negativity. Int J Psychophysiol,81(3),324-331.
    Rabbitt, P. M. (1966). Errors and error correction in choice-response tasks. Journal of Experimental Psychology,71(2),264-272.
    Raedeke, T, D. (1997). Is athlete burnout more than just stress? A sport commitment perspective. Journal of Sport & Exercise Psychology,19(4),396-417.
    Raedeke, T, D., & Smith, A. L. (2001). Development and preliminary validation of an athlete burnout measure. Journal of Sport & Exercise Psychology,23(4),281-306.
    Rudy, B. (1954). Electroencephalographic investigation of conditioned reflex to caffeine in mental fatigue. ActaPhysiolPol,5(3),335-336.
    Sanders, A. F., & Hoogenboom, W. (1970). On effects of continuous active work on performance. Acta Psychologica,33,414-431.
    Santesso, D. L., & Segalowitz, S. J. (2009). The error-related negativity is related to risk taking and empathy in young men. Psychophysiology,46(1),143-152.
    Scheffers, M. K., & Coles, M. G (2000). Performance monitoring in a confusing world: error-related brain activity, judgments of response accuracy, and types of errors. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform,26(1),141-151.
    Scheffers, M. K., Coles, M. G, Bernstein, P., Gehring, W. J., & Donchin, E. (1996). Event-related brain potentials and error-related processing:an analysis of incorrect responses to go and no-go stimuli. Psychophysiology,33(1),42-53.
    Silva, J. M. (1991). An analysis of the training stress syndrome in competitive athletics. Journal of Applied Sport Psychology,2(1),5-20.
    Simon, J. R., & Berbaum, K. (1990). Effect of conflicting cues on information processing:The Stroop effect vs. the Simon effect. Acta Psychologica,73(2),159-170.
    Smith, R. E. (1986). Toward a cognitive-affective model of athletic burnout. Journal of Sport Psychology,8(1),36-50.
    Sosic-Vasic, Z., Ulrich, M., Ruchsow, M., Vasic, N., & Gron, G (2012). The modulating effect of personality traits on neural error monitoring:evidence from event-related FMRI. PLoS One,7(8), e42930.
    Stroop, J. R. (1935). Studies of interference in serial verbal reactions. Journal of Experimental Psychology,18,643-662.
    Suchan, B., Jokisch, D., Skotara, N., & Daum, I. (2007). Evaluation-related frontocentral negativity evoked by correct responses and errors. Behavioural brain research,183(2), 206-212.
    Takahashi, M., Arito, H., & Fukuda, H. (1999). Nurses'workload associated with 16-h night shifts. II:Effects of a nap taken during the shifts. Psychiatry Clin Neurosci,53(2),223-225.
    Tanaka, H., Mochizuki, Y., Masaki, H., Takasawa, N., & Yamazaki, K. (2005). A study of attentional resource of discrete and gradational allocation strategy using error-related negativity (ERN). Shinrigaku Kenkyu,76(1),43-50.
    Tanaka, M., Shigihara, Y., Ishii, A., Funakura, M., Kanai, E., & Watanabe, Y. (2012). Effect of mental fatigue on the central nervous system:an electroencephalography study. Behav Brain Funct,8(1),48-65.
    Taylor, S. F., Stern, E. R., & Gehring, W. J. (2007). Neural systems for error monitoring The Neuroscientist,13(2),160-172.
    Themanson, J. R., & Hillman, C. H. (2006). Cardiorespiratory fitness and acute aerobic exercise effects on neuroelectric and behavioral measures of action monitoring. Neuroscience, 141(2),757-767.
    Themanson, J. R., Pontifex, M. B., Hillman, C. H., & McAuley, E. (2011). The relation of self-efficacy and error-related self-regulation, lnt J Psychophysiol,80(1),1-10.
    Tieges, Z., Richard Ridderinkhof, K., Snel, J., & Kok, A. (2004). Caffeine strengthens action monitoring:evidence from the error-related negativity. Brain Res Cogn Brain Res,21(1), 87-93.
    Tops, M, & Boksem, M. A. (2011). Cortisol involvement in mechanisms of behavioral inhibition. Psychophysiology,48(5),723-732.
    Tops, M., Boksem, M. A., Wester, A. E., Lorist, M. M., & Meijman, T. F. (2006). Task engagement and the relationships between the error-related negativity, agreeableness, behavioral shame proneness and cortisol. Psychoneuroendocrinology,31(7),847-858.
    Turken, A. U., & Swick, D. (2008). The effect of orbitofrontal lesions on the error-related negativity. Neurosci Lett,441(1),7-10.
    Ullsperger, M., & Von Cramon, D. Y. (2001). Subprocesses of performance monitoring:a dissociation of error processing and response competition revealed by event-related fMRI and ERPs. Neuroimage,14(6),1387-1401.
    van der Linden, D., & Eling, P. (2006). Mental fatigue disturbs local processing more than global processing. PsycholRes,70(5),395-402.
    van der Linden, D., Frese, M., & Meijman, T. F. (2003). Mental fatigue and the control of cognitive processes:effects on perseveration and planning. Acta Psychologica,113(1), 45-65.
    van Luijtelaar, G., Verbraak, M., van den Bunt, M., Keijsers, G, & Arns, M. (2010). EEG findings in burnout patients. JNeuropsychiatry Clin Neurosci,22(2),208-217.
    Vidart, L., & Geier, S. (1968). Work, fatigue and mental state in epileptic patients:tele-EEG study. Rev Neurol (Paris),118(6),473.
    Wessel, J. R. (2012). Error awareness and the error-related negativity:evaluating the first decade of evidence. Front Hum Neurosci,6,88.
    Westlye, L. T., Walhovd, K. B., Bjornerud, A., Due-Tonnessen, P., & Fjell, A. M. (2009). Error-related negativity is mediated by fractional anisotropy in the posterior cingulate gyrus-a study combining diffusion tensor imaging and electrophysiology in healthy adults. Cereb Cortex,19(2),293-304.
    Wimmer, F., Hoffmann, R. F., Bonato, R. A., & Moffitt, A. R. (1992). The effects of sleep deprivation on divergent thinking and attention processes. J Sleep Res,1(4),223-230.
    Wiswede, D., Munte, T. F., Goschke, T., & Russeler, J. (2009). Modulation of the error-related negativity by induction of short-term negative affect. Neuropsychologia,47(1),83-90.
    Yeung, N., Botvinick, M. M., & Cohen, J. D. (2004). The neural basis of error detection:conflict monitoring and the error-related negativity. PsycholRev,111(4),931-959.
    Yu, R., & Zhou, X. (2009). To bet or not to bet? The error negativity or error-related negativity associated with risk-taking choices. J Cogn Neurosci,21(4),684-696.
    Zirnheld, P. J., Carroll, C. A., Kieffaber, P. D., O'Donnell, B. F., Shekhar, A., & Hetrick, W. P. (2004). Haloperidol impairs learning and error-related negativity in humans. J Cogn Neurosci,16(6),1098-1112.