用户名: 密码: 验证码:
Attention is required for the perceptual integration of action object pairs
详细信息    查看全文
  • 作者:Nicolas A. McNair ; Irina M. Harris
  • 关键词:Tools ; Action affordance ; Perceptual grouping ; Context effects ; Object recognition ; Attentional load
  • 刊名:Experimental Brain Research
  • 出版年:2016
  • 出版时间:January 2016
  • 年:2016
  • 卷:234
  • 期:1
  • 页码:25-37
  • 全文大小:540 KB
  • 参考文献:Adamo M, Ferber S (2009) A picture says more than a thousand words: behavioural and ERP evidence for attentional enhancements due to action affordances. Neuropsychologia 47:1600–1608PubMed CrossRef
    Allport DA (1968) Phenomenal simultaneity and the perceptual moment hypothesis. Br J Psychol 59:395–406PubMed CrossRef
    Almeida J, Mahon BZ, Nakayama K, Caramazza A (2008) Unconscious processing dissociates along categorical lines. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 105:15214–15218PubMed PubMedCentral CrossRef
    Almeida J, Mahon BZ, Caramazza A (2010) The role of the dorsal visual processing stream in tool identification. Psychol Sci 21(6):772–778PubMed PubMedCentral CrossRef
    Baylis GC, Driver J, Rafal RD (1993) Visual extinction and stimulus repetition. J Cogn Neurosci 5(4):453–466PubMed CrossRef
    Brainard DH (1997) The psychophysics toolbox. Spat Vis 10:433–436PubMed CrossRef
    Buccino G, Sato M, Cattaneo L, Rodà F, Riggio L (2009) Broken affordances, broken objects: a TMS study. Neuropsychologia 47:3074–3078PubMed CrossRef
    Chong TT-J, Williams MA, Cunnington R, Mattingly JB (2008) Selective attention modulates inferior frontal gyrus activity during action observation. Neuroimage 40:298–307PubMed CrossRef
    Creem SH, Proffitt DR (2001) Grasping objects by their handles: a necessary interaction between cognition and action. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 27(1):218–228PubMed CrossRef
    Di Lollo V (1980) Temporal integration in visual memory. J Exp Psychol Gen 109(1):75–97PubMed CrossRef
    Garcea FE, Almeida J, Mahon BZ (2012) A right visual field advantage for visual processing of manipulable objects. Cogn Affect Behav Neurosci 12:813–825PubMed PubMedCentral CrossRef
    Gilchrist ID, Humphreys GW, Riddoch MJ (1996) Grouping and extinction: evidence for low-level modulation of selection. Cogn Neuropsychol 13(8):1223–1249CrossRef
    Green C, Hummel JE (2006) Familiar interacting object pairs are perceptually grouped. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 32(5):1107–1119PubMed CrossRef
    Hogben JH, Di Lollo V (1974) Perceptual integration and perceptual segregation of brief visual stimuli. Vis Res 14:1059–1069PubMed CrossRef
    Humphreys GW, Riddoch MJ (2007) How to define an object: evidence from the effects of action on perception and attention. Mind Lang 22(5):534–547CrossRef
    Humphreys GW, Riddoch MJ, Fortt H (2006) Action relations, semantics relations, and familiarity of spatial position in Balint’s syndrome: crossover effects on perceptual report and on localization. Cogn Affect Behav Neurosci 6(3):236–245PubMed CrossRef
    Johnson-Frey SH (2004) The neural bases of complex tool use in humans. Trends Cogn Sci 8(2):71–78PubMed CrossRef
    Kimchi R, Razpurker-Apfeld I (2004) Perceptual grouping and attention: not all groupings are equal. Psychon Bull Rev 11(4):687–696PubMed CrossRef
    Kimchi R, Yeshurun Y, Cohen-Savransky A (2007) Automatic, stimulus-driven attentional capture by objecthood. Psychon Bull Rev 14(1):166–172PubMed CrossRef
    Kleiner M, Brainard D, Pelli D (2007). What’s new in Psychtoolbox-3? Tutorial session presented at the 30th European conference on visual perception, Arezzo, Italy
    Makris S, Hadar AA, Yarrow K (2011) Viewing objects and planning actions: on the potentiation of grasping behaviours by grasping objects. Brain Cogn 77(2):257–264PubMed CrossRef
    Mattingley JB, Payne JM, Rich A (2006) Attentional load attenuates synaesthetic priming effects in grapheme-colour synaesthesia. Cortex 42(2):213–221PubMed CrossRef
    McNair NA, Harris IM (2014) The conceptual action relationship between a tool and its action recipient modulates their joint perception. Atten Percept Psychophys 76:214–229PubMed CrossRef
    Pelli DG (1997) The VideoToolbox software for visual psychophysics: transforming numbers into movies. Spat Vis 10:437–442PubMed CrossRef
    Pellicano A, Iani C, Borghi AM, Rubichi S, Nicoletti R (2010) Simon-like and functional affordance effects with tools: the effects of object perceptual discrimination and object action state. Q J Exp Psychol 63(11):2190–2201CrossRef
    Pomerantz JR (2003) Wholes, holes, and basic features in vision. Trends Cogn Sci 7(11):471–473PubMed CrossRef
    Rafal R, Danziger S, Grossi G, Machado L, Ward R (2002) Visual detection is gated by attending for action: evidence from hemispatial neglect. Proc Natl Acad Sci 99(25):16371–16375PubMed PubMedCentral CrossRef
    Riddoch MJ, Humphreys GW, Edwards S, Baker T, Wilson K (2003) Seeing the action: neuropsychological evidence for action-based effects on object selection. Nat Neurosci 6(1):82–89PubMed CrossRef
    Riddoch MJ, Humphreys GW, Hickman M, Clift J, Daly A, Colin J (2006) I can see what you are doing: action familiarity and affordance promote recovery from extinction. Cogn Neuropsychol 23(4):583–605PubMed CrossRef
    Riggio L, Patteri I, Oppo A, Buccino G, Umiltà C (2006) The role of affordances in inhibition of return. Psychon Bull Rev 13(6):1085–1090PubMed CrossRef
    Roberts KL, Humphreys GW (2010) The one that does, leads: action relations influence the perceived temporal order of graspable objects. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 36(3):776–780PubMed CrossRef
    Roberts KL, Humphreys GW (2011a) Action relations facilitate the identification of briefly-presented objects. Atten Percept Psychophys 73:597–612PubMed CrossRef
    Roberts KL, Humphreys GW (2011b) Action-related objects influence the distribution of visuospatial attention. Q J Exp Psychol 64(4):669–688CrossRef
    Rorden C, Guerrini C, Swainson R, Lazzeri M, Baylis GC (2008) Event related potentials reveal that increasing perceptual load leads to increased responses for target stimuli and decreased responses for irrelevant stimuli. Front Hum Neurosci 2:4PubMed PubMedCentral CrossRef
    Snodgrass JG, Vanderwart M (1980) A standardized set of 260 pictures: norms for name agreement, image agreement, familiarity, and visual complexity. J Exp Psychol Hum Learn Mem 6(2):174–215CrossRef
    Symes E, Ellis R, Tucker M (2007) Visual object affordances: object orientation. Acta Psychol 124:238–255CrossRef
    Tucker M, Ellis R (2004) Action priming by briefly presented objects. Acta Psychol 116:185–203CrossRef
    Verma A, Brysbaert M (2011) A right visual field advantage for tool recognition in the visual half-field paradigm. Neuropsychologia 49:2342–2348PubMed CrossRef
    Vuilleumier P, Sagiv N (2001) Two eyes make a pair: facial organization and perceptual learning reduce visual extinction. Neuropsychologia 39:1144–1149PubMed CrossRef
    Yoon EY, Humphreys GW, Riddoch MJ (2010) The paired-object affordance effect. J Exp Psychol Hum Perform Percept 36(4):812–824CrossRef
  • 作者单位:Nicolas A. McNair (1)
    Irina M. Harris (1)

    1. School of Psychology, University of Sydney, Sydney, NSW, 2006, Australia
  • 刊物类别:Biomedical and Life Sciences
  • 刊物主题:Biomedicine
    Neurosciences
    Neurology
  • 出版者:Springer Berlin / Heidelberg
  • ISSN:1432-1106
文摘
Previous studies have demonstrated that functionally related objects are perceptually grouped during visual identification if they are depicted as if interacting with each other (Green and Hummel in J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 32(5):1107–1119, 2006). However, it is unclear whether this integration requires attention or occurs pre-attentively. Here, we used a divided-attention task with variable attentional load to address this question. Participants matched a word label to a target object that was immediately preceded by a briefly presented, task-irrelevant tool that was either functionally related or unrelated to the word label (e.g., axe | “log” or hammer | “log”). The tool was either positioned to interact with the target object or faced away from it. The amount of attention available to process the tool was manipulated by asking participants to make a concurrent perceptual discrimination of varying difficulty on a surrounding frame stimulus. The previously demonstrated advantage for the related-and-interacting condition was replicated under conditions of no or low attentional load. This benefit disappeared under high competing attentional load, indicating that attention is required to integrate functionally related objects together into a single perceptual unit. Keywords Tools Action affordance Perceptual grouping Context effects Object recognition Attentional load

© 2004-2018 中国地质图书馆版权所有 京ICP备05064691号 京公网安备11010802017129号

地址:北京市海淀区学院路29号 邮编:100083

电话:办公室:(+86 10)66554848;文献借阅、咨询服务、科技查新:66554700